Here, we present a novel, brief, and active spatial navigation task that assesses both spatial navigation and episodic memory ability. Importantly, spatial navigation and episodic memory were associated with one another, and this task demonstrated sensitivity to exercise.
Spatial navigation (SN) is the ability to locomote through the environment, which requires an understanding of where one is located in time and space. This capacity is known to rely on the sequential firing of place cells within the hippocampus. SN is an important behavior to investigate as this process deteriorates with age, especially in neurodegenerative disorders. However, the investigation of SN is limited by the lack of sophisticated behavioral techniques to assess this hippocampal-dependent task. Therefore, the goal of this protocol was to develop a novel, real-world approach to studying SN in humans. Specifically, an active virtual SN task was developed using a cross-platform game engine. During the encoding phase, participants navigated their way through a virtual city to locate landmarks. During the remembering phase, participants remembered where these reward locations were and delivered items to these locations. Time to find each location was captured and episodic memory was assessed by a free recall phase, including aspects of place, order, item, and association. Movement behavior (x, y, and z coordinates) was assessed through an asset available in the game engine. Importantly, results from this task demonstrate that it accurately captures both spatial learning and memory abilities as well as episodic memory. Further, findings indicate that this task is sensitive to exercise, which improves hippocampal functioning. Overall, the findings suggest a novel way to track human hippocampal functioning over the course of time, with this behavior being sensitive to physical activity training paradigms.
Moving the body through time and space is critical for learning and remembering information about the environment. This ability is known as spatial navigation, and evolutionarily speaking, it is an essential survival tool for locating food, water, social counterparts, and other rewards in the environment1,2. Spatial navigation is dependent upon the hippocampus, a c-shaped limbic system structure in the medial temporal lobe. The hippocampus consists of the CA1, CA2, CA3, and dentate gyrus subregions. The hippocampus supports the encoding, consolidation, and retrieval of memories that help define the conscious experience. Specifically, spatial navigation supports episodic memory, a form of explicit memory that refers to the memory of personal experience, including aspects of time, place, and relevant details associated with the experience (e.g., sights, sounds, smells, emotions). As we spatially navigate through distinct environments, neurons known as place cells systematically fire, enabling us to understand where we are in both time and space. In fact, direct optical stimulation of these neurons has been shown to bias the behavior of rodents towards their physical location (i.e., place fields)3.
Assessing spatial navigation in rodents has traditionally been studied through such behavioral paradigms as the Morris Water Maze, Y maze, T maze, and radial arm maze4,5. Importantly, these behavioral tasks allow for the in vivo investigation of the neural correlates of spatial navigation using techniques such as electrophysiological depth recordings. However, assessing spatial navigation in humans has proven scientifically challenging because most scientific investigations happen in laboratories and not out in the real world. Previous studies in humans have assessed spatial abilities with traditional paper-based tasks such as bi-directional map learning tasks, mental rotation tasks, or spatial memory tasks6,7. Others have utilized computer-based tasks such as the Virtual Morris Water Task or other virtual maze tasks, which have been shown to be correlated with more traditional psychometric measures of spatial ability8,9. Additionally, with the accessibility of publicly available and free video game software packages, researchers have begun to develop 3-dimensional virtual environments that can either be presented on a computer screen or in virtual reality10,11,12,13,14,15. Scientific advances in mobile brain-body imaging (MoBI) have also allowed researchers to begin to explore spatial navigation in real-world settings16,17,18.
Importantly, spatial learning and memory is a cognitive ability that deteriorates with age, with elderly individuals being more likely to lose track of where they are or become lost when they try to return home. This deficit is most likely due to neurodegeneration that occurs at the level of the hippocampus – a highly plastic brain area that is one of the first to deteriorate with age19. Therefore, developing real-world methods to assess spatial navigational and episodic memory abilities is an important avenue of research. At the clinical level, these types of tasks may help determine the progression of memory decline or diagnose mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease, or other forms of dementia. Conversely, physical activity has been identified as one of the best mechanisms to improve spatial navigational abilities. Studies in rodents have shown that exercise enhances learning and memory on various spatial tasks, including the Morris Water Maze, Y maze, T maze, and radial arm maze20. Exercise-induced improvements in spatial abilities have also been demonstrated in humans, with this effect being significantly related to an increase in hippocampal volume7. However, this behavioral effect was demonstrated using a spatial memory task where participants were asked to remember the locations of dots on a screen – a task that may not hold much ecological validity to real-world spatial navigation. Little research has investigated the impact of exercise in humans on spatial navigational tasks presented in virtual environments.
Therefore, a cognitive task was designed to assess spatial learning and memory along with episodic memory using a virtual environment. Importantly, the task was designed using modern-day video game software to enable up-to-date graphic designs and realistic features (e.g., moving clouds in the sky). This task was tested in a group of healthy adults before and after they experienced long-term aerobic exercise practice. Results indicate that participants can encode and remember both spatial information as well as episodic memories regarding their virtual experience. Additionally, findings indicate that performance on this task is plastic, being affected by exercise.
Specifically, a virtual environment was developed through a cross-platform game engine21 that evaluated spatial navigation and episodic memory ability, unique cognitive skills supported by the hippocampus. The map used for this environment was derived from Miller et al. (2013)22. The game engine that was utilized allows developers to download assets to add unique features for the purposes of building virtual environments. An asset23 was utilized that allowed us to build a realistic city environment with roads and buildings through which participants could navigate. Additionally, an asset24 was used that allowed tracking of participants' x, y, and z coordinates and rotation while they traveled through the virtual environment. The aforementioned asset allowed the recording of these features on a millisecond timescale (~33 ms). The virtual environment was then compiled and administered as a spatial navigation task that participants could complete at home on a laptop or desktop computer. The below protocol details how to administer and engage with this spatial navigation task.
All study documentation and data collection methods were approved by and in compliance with the New York University Committee on Activities Involving Human Subjects. Participants gave their informed consent prior to participating in any study-related activities.
1. Setting up gameplay
2. Recording brain activity with electroencephalography (EEG) during the spatial navigation task
NOTE: EEG measures the activity of neurons in the cortex of the human brain in microvolts on a millisecond timescale through electrodes placed on the scalp. EEG is a noninvasive form of brain imaging that allows a participant's brain to be scanned while they conduct other activities, such as navigating virtual environments.
Figure 1: Electroencephalography recording during spatial navigation gameplay. (A) Image of a person equipped with a mobile electroencephalography (EEG) device while performing the spatial navigation task. Power spectral density plot of theta activity (4-8 Hz) during the (B) encoding phase, (C) remembering phase, and (D) episodic memory phase. All data was preprocessed and power normalized by frequency (uV2/Hz). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
NOTE: Researchers using Arduino technologies may also be able to send synchronized triggers between the EEG recording and the game-engine environment so that an exact pairing between the neurophysiological and behavioral data can occur on a millisecond timescale. With these markers, researchers will be able to refer back to the participant's brain activity before, during, and after critical interactions with the virtual environment. Researchers may also consider conducting a period of baseline brain activity before and/or after engagement with the virtual environment so that later comparisons can be made.
3. Instructions for the spatial navigation task (Figure 2)
Figure 2: Images of spatial navigation task. Screenshots of the spatial navigation and episodic memory task developed in a cross-platform game engine. Example screenshots are presented from left to right, starting at the top left corner: (A) overall instructions; (B) travel during encoding phase; (C) locating storefront during the encoding phase; (D) travel during encoding phase; (E) instructions for remembering phase; (F) delivery portion of remembering phase; (G) instructions for episodic memory phase; (H) episodic memory phase; (I) end of game. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
4. Encoding phase of the spatial navigation task
Figure 3: Images of storefronts. Participants visited five of the eighteen locations developed in the environment, each with a unique and detailed storefront. Examples of these locations included (A) a pizza parlor, (B) a vitamin shop, (C) a furniture store, (D) a wedding store, (E) a kiosk, and (F) a casino. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4: Map of spatial navigation task. Bird's eye view of the virtual environment through which participants navigated. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
5. Remembering phase of the spatial navigation task
6. Episodic memory phase of the spatial navigation task
NOTE: Episodic memory testing will take place after the remembering phase is complete.
7. Completing the task
8. Data collection and analysis
Description of gameplay from a coding perspective: For the "encoding" phase, a series of eighteen waypoints were placed around the 3-dimensional space, with each having an associated "Delivery Item" (i.e., item to deliver to the location). References to these waypoints were stored in the player controller and ordered statically prior to beginning the task; that is, if the pizza shop was placed in position one, it would always be in position one at first. In order to provide some degree of randomness to the waypoints that participants encountered, the waypoint list was shuffled via the Fisher-Yates shuffle algorithm. The Fisher-Yates shuffle, as implemented for this study, generates a pseudorandom permutation of the original sequence in place. Any possible permutation can be generated with equal likeliness. The algorithm begins by selecting an element from the end of the list (n). A pseudorandom number is generated in the range of [0, n] and assigned to the value k. The nth value is then swapped with the kth value. Next, the value of n is decremented by one, and the process repeats until there is only a single index not yet considered.
After the list of waypoints was shuffled, the first five elements were selected. Optimal paths were generated via the game engine's navigation mesh system and built-in optimal path calculations. This series of paths began at the starting location of the participant and created a linked chain between each of the waypoints, terminating at the final waypoint. When the participants gained control, they were directed to follow these paths, designated by a green line and a moving arrow that provided intended direction information. Though this green line and moving arrow were provided, participants were able to actively navigate throughout the virtual environment. When the participant entered the bounds of the waypoint, the displayed path was swapped with the next path in the list.
Upon visiting the intended number of waypoint elements, the participant entered the "remembering" phase (termed RevisitIntermission in the code), where they were directed to revisit the landmarks in the order that they were previously shown. As the participant attempted to revisit the locations presented during the guided tour, they were presented with an image specified by the waypoints' associated "Delivery Item". They were not presented with a suggested path. Their movements were tracked with an object motion tracker component sourced from the asset store.
When the participants finished traveling to each presented waypoint, they were given instructions directing them to the next screen to recall the locations they had visited and the items delivered to each one. During the recall phase, the participants were presented with a prompt with two text entries. The first dictated the waypoint the participant was asked to travel to. The second dictated the "Delivery Item" associated with this waypoint. Response and response time were recorded for each prompt.
At the end of the task, the data were collected and stored in JSON representation. The first section recorded the revisit phase, where participants were asked to find locations without the aid of a guiding line. Recorded values included the waypoint name, the "Delivery Item" name, and the time it took to arrive at the waypoint. The second section recorded the responses presented during the recall phase. This section included participant responses for location, "Delivery Item", and the time it took to respond to the aforementioned prompts. All code can be found and downloaded at https://github.com/embodiedbrainlab/BassoSpatialNavigationTask.
Power analysis and statistics: A correlation point biserial model power analysis was conducted with G*Power 3.1 using a two-tailed test, an effect size of 0.3, alpha level of 0.05, and a power of 0.8 to determine a sample size of n = 8226. Descriptive statistics were used to assess participants' age, number of cycling classes, and general measures, including both spatial navigation and episodic memory abilities. An independent samples t-test was utilized to test significant differences between the total number of workouts between experimental and control groups. Considering that not all data were normally distributed, as assessed by Shapiro-Wilk's test (p<0.05), we utilized the non-parametric Spearman's rho correlation coefficient to assess relationships between spatial navigation and episodic memory abilities as well as age and spatial navigation abilities. An alpha value of 0.05 was utilized to determine statistical significance. Bonferroni corrections were used in a family of statistical tests where appropriate. IBM SPSS Statistics Version 26 was utilized for all statistical analyses. Pearson's product-moment correlation was utilized to assess the relationship between the total number of cycling workouts and spatial navigation abilities, as this was the procedure conducted by Basso et al. (2022)27.
Participants: N = 130 participants were recruited from Austin, TX, through various techniques, including online and flyer advertisements. Inclusion criteria included having English as their primary language and being between the ages of 25-55 years of age (average 30.16 ± 0.49). Additionally, all participants needed to report being physically healthy and having a moderate and regular exercise regimen (defined as exercising one or two times per week for 20 min or more for the past 3 months). Exclusion criteria included being a current smoker or preexisting physical health conditions that made exercise difficult or unsafe. Exclusion criteria also included having a current diagnosis of and/or taking medication for psychiatric or neurological conditions, including anxiety, depression, bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, or epilepsy.
For pre-intervention data, n = 11 participants were missing data due to technical issues, and n = 1 participant was excluded due to non-task adherence, leaving a total of n = 117 participants for analysis. Of the n = 80 participants that completed the three-month exercise regimen, n = 11 participants did not complete the final spatial navigation task, leaving a total of n = 69 participants for analysis of post-intervention and repeated measures data. This smaller sample size was utilized to examine the relationship between the number of cycling sessions and spatial navigation abilities. The control group engaged in 20.73 (± 0.72) workouts over the course of the intervention, whereas the experimental group engaged in 47.87 (± 2.24) workouts, which represented a statistically significant difference (t[45.76] = −11.554, p < 0.001).
General measures and their relationships: This new virtual environment task measures both spatial navigation and episodic memory capacity. During the initial pre-intervention testing period, the task took an average of 318.69 (±21.56) s to complete, with the average seek time for each of the five sites being 82.88 (±5.19) s (Figure 5A); these data points represent spatial navigation ability (i.e., spatial learning and memory). Additionally, participants were able to encode place, item, order, and association aspects of the virtual experience, with participants remembering 14.84 (±0.37) out of 20 novel experiences in their environment (Figure 5B); these data points represent episodic memory ability. Importantly, the total time (Figure 6A; r = -0.314, p < 0.001) and average seek time (Figure 6B; r = -0.286, p < 0.001) were significantly correlated with the episodic memory score, indicating that spatial navigation ability is associated with episodic memory in this task.
Figure 5: Time of task. Mean (± SEM) for both (A) spatial navigational ability represented in both average seek time and total seek time (provided in seconds) and (B) episodic memory ability represented in the encoding and remembering of place, item, order, association and overall episodic memory score. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 6: Relationship of spatial navigation ability to episodic memory. Improved spatial navigation ability as represented by shorter (A) average seek time and (B) total seek time is associated with enhanced episodic memory as represented by the episodic memory score. *p < 0.001. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
X and z coordinates represented in virtual space: Using an object motion tracker asset, x and z coordinates were tracked in this 3-dimensional virtual space (Supplementary File 1). As moving up and down in the game (i.e., jumping) is not enabled in this spatial navigation task, y coordinates did not provide useful information. However, the x and z coordinates enabled us to assess how the participant moved throughout the game. Based on this data, computer code was designed to visually display a heat map of where the participant traveled throughout the map. Figure 7 displays a heat map from one representative participant, which highlights the route the participant took during the remembering phase. The spots that are highlighted in yellow/red correspond to the delivery (i.e., reward) locations on the map.
Figure 7: Occupancy heat map. Occupancy heat map demonstrating the route of the participant. Yellow/red sections of the graph represent those locations where the participant frequented and correspond to places in the spatial navigation task where participants had to deliver items (i.e., reward locations). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Relationship between age and spatial navigation abilities: Initial investigations indicated that spatial navigation ability as assessed by total seek time was significantly associated with age (Figure 8; r = 0.157, p = 0.045). As age increases, spatial navigation ability decreases, as evidenced by an increased total seek time. However, when the Bonferroni correction was applied, with statistical significance being assessed at p = 0.025 for two correlations (i.e., total seek time and average seek duration), the correlation was no longer significant.
Figure 8: Relationship of spatial navigation ability to age. When evaluated using a Bonferonni correction (p < 0.025), age was not significantly associated with spatial navigation ability as represented by total seek time. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Relationship between aerobic exercise training and spatial navigation abilities: Aerobic exercise training took place at an indoor cycling studio28. All classes were 45 min in duration and included cycling at moderate to vigorous intensities throughout the duration of the class. Participants underwent random assignment to either maintain their existing exercise regimen or increase their exercise regimen. Participants who maintained their exercise regimen engaged in 1 to 2 classes per week, whereas participants who increased their exercise regimen engaged in 4 to 7 classes per week. Participants engaged in their assigned exercise regimen for a period of 3 months. Spatial navigation and episodic memory ability were tested before and after exercise training. Additional details of the intervention can be found in Basso et al. (2022)27. The total number of cycling classes over the course of three months was significantly associated with average seek duration (Figure 9A; r = -0.321, p = 0.007) and total seek time (Figure 9B; r = -0.242, p = 0.045). However, when the Bonferroni correction was applied, with statistical significance being assessed at p = 0.025 for two correlations (i.e., total seek time and average seek duration), the correlation for total seek time was no longer significant. Additional findings from the intervention can be found in Basso et al. (2022)27.
Figure 9: Relationship of spatial navigation ability to exercise. An increased number of cycling sessions is associated with improved spatial navigation ability as represented by (A) average seek time and (B) total seek time. *p < 0.05. This figure has been modified with permission from Basso et al.27. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Supplementary File 1: Raw data 1. Raw data, including information regarding the remembering (revisit) and episodic memory (recall) phase of the spatial navigation task. Data regarding the participant's x and z coordinates from traveling around the 3-dimensional virtual space during the encoding and remembering phases of the experiment is also presented. Please click here to download this File.
Supplementary File 2: Raw data 2. Raw data with calculations (presented in red) to determine start time, end time, average seek duration, place score, item score, order score, association score, and episodic memory score. Please click here to download this File.
This study examined the effectiveness of a novel virtual reality task in assessing spatial navigation in humans. This cognitive task, which only takes about 10 min to complete, can be utilized to assess two unique types of hippocampal-dependent cognition – spatial navigation and episodic memory ability. Importantly, spatial navigational ability was significantly associated with episodic memory ability. Finally, this task was sensitive to a physical activity training paradigm. That is, increased exercise was associated with increased performance. This task was inspired by the work of Miller et al. (2013) who investigated virtual environments in patients with drug-resistant epilepsy and hippocampal depth electrodes placed for the purpose of seizure localization. They found that during the familiarization phase of the spatial navigation task (i.e., the encoding phase), place-responsive cells in the hippocampus and associated medial temporal lobe structures became activated22. Additionally, they found that when participants were engaged in a free recall component (i.e., a remembering phase that did not involve active navigation), the same place-responsive cells that were active during encoding became active once again. Extant studies in rodents utilizing open field and maze-like environments have shown the existence of such place cells, with Drs. John O'Keefe, May-Britt Moser, and Edvard Moser winning the 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine for this discovery2,29,30,31. Additionally, studies using virtual environments in humans have shown that similar cells in the human hippocampus encode travel through time and space22,32,33. Though the task is similar to the one presented in Miller et al. (2013) and others22,34,35,36,37,38, it was developed with the most current cross-platform game engine and technologies, utilizing real-world features such as moving clouds and clear city landmarks and storefront features. Other researchers have utilized other spatial navigation tasks in humans; however, these tasks are limited in their ecological validity. For instance, the virtual Starmaze task is used to assess navigation abilities but places participants in a star-shaped maze39,40,41,42,43,44. Moreover, NavWell is an accessible platform that hosts spatial navigation and memory experiments akin to the Morris Water Maze in rodents (placing participants in a circular arena), and provides developers with basic geometric shapes to build an environment45. Additionally, the Landmarks assets on cross-platform game engines are available for building and developing spatial navigation tasks that exist in a square setting12. The present task is unique in that it provides users with a setting and task similar to the real world – navigating a cityscape and memorizing landmarks and actions. The task is also different from the virtual Starmaze task and NavWell because it assesses episodic memory in addition to spatial navigation.
In this task, spatial navigation ability was significantly related to episodic memory ability. Others have shown that these two cognitive abilities are indeed distinct and that they rely on different regions of the hippocampus38,46. The popular "Cognitive Map Theory" states that the brain builds and stores a "map" of an individual's spatial environment so that it can later be used in the future to guide actions and behaviors47. Research has suggested that the hippocampus encodes spatial information while also supporting episodic memory formation. More specifically, it is thought that the right hippocampus encodes spatial memory while the left hippocampus stores episodic memories38. Results of the current novel spatial navigation task, which demonstrate a clear link between spatial and episodic memory, lend support to the Cognitive Map Theory and suggest that this task could potentially be used to examine the relationship between spatial navigation and episodic memory in non-clinical populations. Future studies should seek to examine this relationship in clinical populations, including those with neurodegenerative disorders such as mild cognitive impairment, Alzheimer's disease, or other types of dementia.
This task was sensitive to exercise or the total amount of cycling sessions engaged in over a 3-month period. Previous studies in rodents have shown that exercise is one of the most potent ways to increase hippocampal-dependent cognition, including long-term memory, pattern separation, spontaneous alternation, contextual fear conditioning, passive avoidance learning, and novel object recognition, with this effect being dependent on exercise-induced increases in hippocampal neurogenesis48,49,50. Additionally, the literature has shown that long-term exercise enhances hippocampal functioning in humans, with improvements seen in word list recall, story recall, and both spatial and non-spatial relational memory; this effect is thought to be driven by exercise-induced increases in hippocampal volume7,27,51,52,53,54,55. This new spatial navigation task complements the rodent findings and adds to the human literature, showing the importance of physical activity for spatial navigation abilities.
Though in initial investigations, age was negatively associated with spatial navigation ability, this effect was eliminated when applying a Bonferroni correction. This indicates that spatial navigation ability may be preserved through the age of 55. Other literature demonstrates that spatial navigation is a cognitive ability that declines with age56,57,58. Neuroimaging studies have revealed that age-related neurodegeneration in areas including the hippocampus, parahippocampal gyrus, posterior cingulate cortex (retrosplenial cortex), parietal lobes, and prefrontal cortex may be involved in such age-related cognitive decline58. Considering that the age range was limited (25-55 years of age), by including a larger age range, especially older adults (65+), future researchers may see a significant correlation between age and spatial navigation ability. Future studies should consider conducting this spatial navigation task in adults 65 years and older and even those with mild cognitive impairment or other dementia-like disorders.
One obvious missing link in virtual navigation tasks is the lack of the body-brain relationship. That is, in navigating through real-world environments, the activation occurs at the level of the peripheral and central nervous systems, including activation of the proprioceptors, exteroceptors, interoceptors, and vestibular system along with the sensori-motor cortices, basal ganglia, and cerebellum. Without this physical input, virtual navigation may be distinctly different from physical navigation. Despite this, studies have shown that virtual environments stimulate the same brain regions as real-world navigation22,32,33. Making the task more active, as was the design in the current task, may help convince the brain that it is physically moving through time and space, mimicking natural spatial navigation. Others have found support for this hypothesis. A study by Meade et al. (2019) examined the differences between active and passive encoding while using a similar virtual spatial navigation task59. Active navigation refers to participants being able to move on their own through the virtual space (akin to the present study), while passive navigation consists of a guided tour where participants do not move but are rather shown the navigational route. The authors suggested that active navigation may be more beneficial for older populations due to the involvement of physical (e.g., locomotion and proprioception) and cognitive components (e.g., decision-making and attention), and may serve to enhance memory performance through direct involvement in the process of memory encoding. The active navigation utilized in the present study could explain the results, demonstrating that participants were able to accurately recall episodic memories of their experiences.
Active navigation may also help to engage multisensory integration areas such as the retrosplenial complex (RSC)60,61,62. A recent study found that actual ambulation during a virtual reality spatial navigation task requiring participants to travel between locations while remembering home and landmark locations resulted in RSC theta oscillations (i.e., 4-8 hertz neuronal oscillations recorded with EEG)16. This increased theta power was most prominent during head direction changes and rotations. In rodents, it has been shown that RSC theta activity is essential to spatial coding involving grid cells and head direction computation63,64. The RSC is also thought to be important for using cues from the environment to anchor a human's cognitive map47.
While virtual spatial navigation tasks provide many benefits, they preclude the individual from physically moving through time and space, causing limited activation of the proprioceptive, vestibular, and sensori-motor systems. An incongruence exists between sensory and motor processes, which may cause some participants to become dizzy or nauseous. In the present task, this was limited by controlling the speed at which participants were able to move through and look around the environment. To be able to encode all aspects of the environment, it was necessary to be able to look around (i.e., engage in virtual head rotation); however, this ability needed to be slow enough to ensure that participants did not get physically ill. Despite this, the ability to spatially navigate while sedentary is advantageous in that it allows researchers to study individuals who experience mobility issues, physical fatigue, or other disabilities that prevent an individual from being ambulatory. Another limitation is that this task has not yet been tested for reliability and validity, while other tasks are moving in this direction, including the virtual spatial navigation assessment (VSNA)65. Future research could examine associated neural activity through electroencephalography or functional magnetic resonance imaging while participants complete this spatial navigation task. Participants could also be fitted with devices measuring physiological variables such as heart rate variability and electrodermal activity. This would allow for an examination of both the peripheral and central mechanisms that occur while navigating virtual environments. Importantly, this task can be used to assess changes in spatial navigation ability over time. Future studies could utilize this task to investigate how aging or neurodegenerative conditions such as Alzheimer's or Parkinson's disease affect an individual's spatial navigation and episodic memory. Conversely, this task could be used to explore how additional mind-body-movement interventions affect spatial navigation and episodic memory, including dance, yoga, or meditation.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
This work was supported by the iTHRIV Scholars Program, which is supported in part by the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the NIH (UL1TR003015 and KL2TR003016). We would like to acknowledge Dr. Samuel McKenzie, Michael Astolfi, Meet Parekh, and Andrei Marks for their computer programming contributions.
Unity Real-Time Development Platform | Unity | Unity Student / Unity Personal | https://unity.com/ |