We present three protocols that assess different forms of impulsivity in rats and other small mammals. Intertemporal choice procedures evaluate the tendency to discount the value of delayed outcomes. Differential reinforcement of low rates and feature-negative discrimination evaluate response inhibition capacity with and without punishment for inappropriate responses, respectively.
The present article provides a guide for the conduction and analysis of three conditioning-based protocols to evaluate impulsivity in rats. Impulsivity is a meaningful concept because it is associated with psychiatric conditions in humans and with maladaptive behavior in non-human animals. It is believed that impulsivity is composed of separate factors. There are laboratory protocols devised to assess each of these factors using standardized automated equipment. Delay discounting is associated with the incapacity to be motivated by delayed outcomes. This factor is evaluated through intertemporal choice protocols, which consist of presenting the individual with a choice situation involving an immediate reward and a larger but delayed reward. Response inhibition deficit is associated with the incapacity to withhold prepotent responses. Differential reinforcement of low rates (DLR) and feature-negative discrimination protocols assess the response inhibition deficit factor of impulsivity. The former imposes a condition to a motivated individual in which most wait a minimum period of time for a response to be rewarded. The latter evaluates the capacity of individuals to refrain from food seeking responses when a signal of the absence of food is presented. The purpose of these protocols is to construct an objective quantitative measure of impulsivity, which serves to make cross-species comparisons, allowing the possibility of translational research. The advantages of these particular protocols include their easy set-up and application, which stems from the relatively small amount of equipment needed and the automated nature of these protocols.
Impulsivity can be conceptualized as a behavioral dimension associated with maladaptive outcomes1. Despite the widespread use of this term, there is no universal consensus on its precise definition. In fact, several authors have defined impulsivity by giving examples of impulsive behaviors or their consequences, rather than delineating which distinctive aspects govern the phenomenon. For instance, impulsivity is assumed to involve an inability to wait, plan, inhibit prepotent behaviors, or an insensitivity to delayed outcomes2, and it has been considered a core vulnerability to addictive behavior3. Bari and Robbins4 have characterized impulsivity as the co-occurrence of strong impulses, being triggered by dispositional and situational variables, and dysfunctional inhibitory processes. A different definition was provided by Dalley and Robbins, who stated that impulsivity could be regarded as a predisposition to rapid, often premature, actions without appropriate insight5. Yet, another definition of impulsivity, proposed by Sosa and dos Santos6, is a behavior tendency that deviates an organism from maximizing available rewards due to the acquired control exerted over the organism's responding by stimuli incidentally related to those rewards.
Due to the behavioral processes related to impulsivity, its neurophysiological substrate involves structures in common with those of motivated behavior, decision making and reward valuing. This is supported by studies that show that structures of the cortico-striatal pathway (e.g., nucleus accumbens [NAc], prefrontal cortex [PFC], amygdala, and caudate putamen [CPU]), as well as the ascending monoaminergic neurotransmitter system, participate in the expression of impulsive behavior7. However, the neural substrate of impulsivity is more complex than that. Although NAc and PFC are involved in impulsive behavior, these structures are part of a more complex system, and also are composed by substructures that have different functions (for more detailed documentation, see Dalley and Robbins5).
Regardless of the controversies about its nature and biological substrate, this behavioral dimension is known to vary across individuals, in which case it can be considered as a trait, and within individuals, in which case it can be considered as a state8. Impulsivity has long been recognized as a feature of some psychiatric conditions, such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), substance abuse, and manic episodes9. There seems to be a high consensus that impulsivity is composed by multiple dissociable factors, including unwillingness to wait (i.e., delay discounting), incapacity to refrain prepotent responses (i.e., inhibitory deficit), difficulty to focus on relevant information (i.e., inattention), and a tendency to engage in risky situations (i.e., sensation seeking)5,10,11. Each of these factors can be assessed through special behavioral tasks, which are usually assigned to two broad categories: choice and response inhibition (these may have different labels between each authors' taxonomies). Some important features of such behavioral tasks are that they could be applied across several animal species2 and that they allow studying impulsivity in controlled laboratory conditions.
Modeling a behavioral dimension with laboratory non-human animals has a number of advantages including the possibility of measuring specific, operationalized behavioral tendencies, allowing the researchers to largely reduce confounding variables (e.g., contamination by past life events4) and to implement experimental manipulations such as chronic pharmacological administration, performing neurotoxic lesions, or genetic manipulations. Most of these protocols have analogue versions for humans, which make comparisons easy5. Importantly, using analogues of these laboratory protocols in humans is effective to aid diagnosis of psychiatric conditions, such as ADHD (especially when more than one protocol is applied12).
Like any other psychological measurement, laboratory protocols for assessing impulsivity must comply with particular criteria in order to achieving the goal of providing insight into the phenomenon under study. To be considered as an appropriate model of impulsive behavior a laboratory protocol should be reliable, and possess (at least, in some degree) face, construct, and/or predictive validity13. Reliability could imply either that an effect upon the measurement would replicate if a manipulation is conducted two or more times, or that the measurement is consistent over time or across different situations14,15. The former feature would be especially useful for experimental studies, while the latter would be so for correlational studies14. Face validity refers to the degree in which what is measured resembles the phenomenon that is supposed to be modeled, as to being, for example, affected by the same variables. Predictive validity refers to the ability of a measure to forecast future performance in protocols, which aim to measure the same or a related construct. Finally, construct validity refers to whether the protocol reproduces behavior that is theoretically sound regarding the process or processes assumed to be involved in the phenomenon under study. However, although these are highly desirable features, one should be cautious when stating that a protocol is valid purely based on these criteria16.
There are several protocols to measure impulsivity in laboratory settings. However, the present article presents only three such methods: intertemporal choice, differential reinforcement of low rates, and feature-negative discrimination. Intertemporal procedures aim to assess the delay discounting (i.e., the difficulty of delayed outcomes to control behavior) component of impulsivity. The basic rationale of this protocol is confronting subjects with two rewards that differ in both magnitude and delay17. One alternative provides a small immediate reward (termed smaller sooner, SS) and the other provides a larger but delayed reward (termed larger later, LL). The proportion of responses to the SS alternative can be used as an index of impulsivity18. In differential reinforcement of low rates procedures, the factor of impulsivity to be assessed is response inhibition (i.e., incapacity to withhold prepotent responses) when there is a negative punishment contingency upon inappropriate responding. The rationale of this protocol is introducing subjects to a situation in which the only way of obtaining rewards is to pause their responding19. Finally, feature-negative discrimination procedure evaluates response inhibition when there is no explicit punishment upon inappropriate responding. The rationale of this protocol (also known as Pavlovian conditioned inhibition or the A+/AX- procedure) is to evaluate subjects' ability to withhold unnecessary responses20.
These procedures stand out in comparison to others as having some convenient features. For example, the procedures presented here are suitable for being conducted in minimally equipped conditioning chambers (also known as 'the Skinner box'). Figure 1 shows a diagram of a typical conditioning chamber. Conditioning chambers are useful research instruments due to a number of advantages. They allow automated collection of a relatively large volume of data, maximizing the number of subjects assessed for unity of time and space21. Moreover, behavioral studies conducted in conditioning chambers require minimal researcher intervention, which reduces the time and effort invested by laboratory staff, unlike other available methods (e.g., non-automated T-mazes, set-shifting boxes)21. Minimizing researchers' intervention also help in reducing researchers' bias, decreasing effects of researchers' learning curve, and a reduction of handling-induced stress22. Typical conditioning chambers are fairly standardized to be used with medium sized rodents, such as rats (R. norvegicus), but can be employed to study other taxa, like similar-sized marsupials (e.g., D. albiventris, and L. crassicaudata23). There are also commercial conditioning chambers adapted for smaller (e.g., mice [M. musculus]) and larger (e.g., non-human primates) species. Setting up and conducting the protocols presented in this article require minimal programming skills and demand a quite low number of attainable input and output devices, unlike more sophisticated alternative methods (e.g., 5-choice serial reaction time task [5-CSRTT]24 and sign-tracking25).
Figure 1: Diagram of a conditioning chamber prototype. The main components of the conditioning chamber include: (1) left lever, (2) food receptacle (equipped with lateral infrared diodes to detect head entries), (3) focalized light, (4) speaker for tone emission (rear view), (5) house light (rear view), (6) food dispenser. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
The three protocols described in this section require the use of rats as subjects. Most laboratory rat strains are suitable; for example, Wistar, Long-Evans, Sprague-Dawley, etc. The Ethics Committee of the Universidad Iberoamericana, following the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources, Commission on Life Sciences, National Research Council, 1996), approved the laboratory protocols to be described.
1. Animal Housing and Preparation
2. Preliminary Training
NOTE: Before starting any of these behavioral protocols rats need to become accustomed to the conditioning chambers and food pellets. It is also vital to train the responses with which the animals would operate in the protocol. The three protocols presented here use appetitive motivation to induce behavior indicative of impulsiveness, like most other available alternative tasks (with select exceptions28). Conventional food dispensers are well suited to deliver both commercial refined grain and sugar pellets but can even handle “raw” grain under certain circumstances29.
3. Programming Automated Protocols
NOTE: The used values (e.g., delays, reward amounts, number of trials, session durations, schedules’ values, time-out length, inter-trial interval span, threshold for forced trials, presence/absence of accompanying stimuli, stimuli durations) presented were arbitrarily selected. Readers may want to consult the literature for determining appropriate parameters and conditions for accomplishing their particular goals. Codes for conducting samples of the three protocols presented here in a MED-PC environment are provided in the repository that can be found in the following URL: https://github.com/SaavedraPablo/MED-PC-codes. Such codes can be freely downloaded and modified according to particular needs.
Figure 2: Diagram of input and output events in two consecutive trials of an intertemporal choice procedure. Diagram of a prototypical intertemporal choice procedure, illustrating an SS alternative choice and an LL alternative choice, in two consecutive trials. Each row depicts the timeline of occurrence of particular output or input events. Spikes in the SS timeline represent choices of the smaller-sooner alternative (upon the accomplishment of the variable-interval schedule). Spikes in the LL timeline represent choices of the larger later alternative (idem). Asterisks in the Rw timeline represent reward deliveries. Elevated plateaus in the OR timeline represent periods of opportunity to respond (they are usually signaled, and its duration varies depending on the time that the individual takes to accomplish to the specified criterion); TO stands for the timeout that begins after reward delivery and ends with the next trial; during this period both levers are retracted. Note that timeout durations vary depending on the type of trial (SS choice or LL choice) in order to keep inter-trial intervals equated. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 3: Diagram of a hypothetical response pattern and its programmed consequences in a DRL 15 s procedure. Spikes in the R timeline represent the timeline of responses spontaneously emitted by the subject. Asterisks in the Rw timeline represent the timeline of reward deliveries. Numbers below the Cl row represent a clock counting down from 15 s the amount of time remaining before the next opportunity to respond and earning a reward. Note that reward delivery only occurs if a response is given since a minimum time of 15 s has elapsed from the last response. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4: Diagram of the types of trial used in the feature-negative discrimination procedure. Elevations in the A timeline represent onsets of the excitatory stimulus. Elevations in the X timeline represent onsets on the inhibitory stimulus. Asterisks in the food timeline represent food delivery. (A) A+ trials include the presentation of the excitatory stimulus followed by food delivery. (B) AX- trials include the presentation of the excitatory stimulus in compound with the inhibitory stimulus without food delivery. Recall that trials must be interspersed randomly and set apart by relatively long inter-trial intervals for better results. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
4. Running the Protocols
5. Data Collection and Analysis
NOTE: Codes for extracting and manipulating data from MED-PC output files (saved with the extension .txt) for each procedure are provided in the repository that can be found in the following URL: https://github.com/SaavedraPablo/MED-PC-to-R-codes.
Figure 5: Histogram of IRTs for one rat in a single session on the DRL 10 s protocol. The distribution is bimodal, with one of the peaks at very short IRTs (burst responses) and the other localized near the time criterion of the protocol (timed responses). Note as well that there is an accumulation of a small number of responses to the right and relatively far from the timed distribution (attentional lapses). Data was extracted from the 9th session in the DRL protocol of Rat 6 in a recent unpublished study. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
The three protocols described in this article may be each conducted alone or in conjunction with other procedures; this will depend on the research question, which in turn will determine the study design. Some examples of study designs that are compatible with these protocols are: (1) time series studies, which aim to describe longitudinal changes in performance; (2) quantification of individual variability, which aims to determine the reliability of the measures; (3) cross-sectional correlation studies, which aims to evaluate whether performance in one protocol can be used to predict performance on another protocol conducted afterwards; (4) longitudinal correlation studies, which aim to ascertain whether performance in one protocol can be used to predict performance on another protocol conducted concurrently; (5) non-experimental group comparisons, which aim to assess whether two or more samples from different populations differ with regards to impulsive performance; (6) pretest-posttest comparisons, which aim to determine whether an intervention (e.g., behavioral, pharmacological, surgical) is effective in altering (e.g., increase, decrease, stabilize) impulsive performance; (7) experimental simple group comparisons, which aim to evaluate whether an intervention if effective in altering impulsive performance but pretest measuring is not available (e.g., in interventions made in early stages of development intended to impact in adult performance). This list is not intended to be exhaustive and combinations of study designs are possible and encouraged.
As stated above, the intertemporal choice procedure is designed to assess the delay-discounting component of impulsivity. The remaining two protocols are supposed to examine inhibitory capacity, which is assumed to be one of the core components of impulsivity. DRL protocols evaluate response inhibition when inappropriate responding is explicitly punished by reward omission. On the other hand, feature-negative discrimination assesses response inhibition when there is no nominal punishment contingency for inappropriate responses. Next, some representative results of one of each protocol from the present laboratory or elsewhere are described.
Figure 6 shows a comparison of performance in an intertemporal choice procedure from a sample of spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) and Wistar rats. The former is a widely accepted rat strain model of ADHD, while the latter is a usual control strain. The SS alternative delivered a single food pellet after a 2 s fixed interval schedule and the LL alternative delivered four food pellets after a 28 s fixed interval schedule (recall that these alternatives were available upon accomplishment of an initial schedule of reinforcement; in this case a variable interval of 30 s). As depicted, the log ratio of lever response rate associated with the SS alternative is higher in SHR compared to Wistar rats. This can be interpreted as SHR presenting a preference for the immediate reward at the expense of a richer but delayed alternative, a sign of high delay-discounting related impulsivity.
Figure 6: Comparison of preference for Alternative SS in an intertemporal choice procedure for SHR and Wistar Rats. The Y axis displays the log-transformed SS/LL ratios. Boxplots are constituted by data from the average of the last five sessions performance for a group of eight SHR and a group of eight Wistar Rats. Data was adapted from the study conducted by Orduña37 (Figure 2 and Figure 3) with the author's permission. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Regarding performance on DRL protocols, Figure 7 shows longitudinal data of a single rat with a 10 s temporal restraint on responding. As it can be seen, during the first sessions the rat emits a high proportion of burst responses but there is a decrease on further sessions. It also may be seen that in earlier sessions there are few responses near the temporal criterion of the protocol. However, as the animal acquires experience in the task, it eventually learns to respond around 10 s. This represents evidence of the role of learning in performance in this protocol. Note, however, that none of the IRTs lower than 10 s were rewarded; even in the 18th session, there is a great proportion of ineffective responses. Such a performance denotes an important quality of the protocol: at least with these parameters, the task is not easy to master, which is helpful in avoiding problems associated with ceiling effects.
Figure 7: Longitudinal progression of performance on a DRL protocol for one rat. Each of the stacked plots displays the estimate of the probability density distribution of IRTs for one subject (Rat 2) along 18 sessions. Data was extracted from a recent unpublished study. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
An example of a pharmacological effect on DRL performance is shown in Figure 8. After reaching a steady performance in a DRL procedure with a target time of 10 s, five female rats received a 1 mL/kg subcutaneous injection of saline and were tested in the same procedure 30 min later for eight consecutive days. Then, saline was replaced with an equal volume of 0.05 mg/kg haloperidol and performance was tested for six more sessions. This aimed at testing whether impulsive performance in this procedure was decreased via D2 receptors antagonism. The dose was selected because it is known that haloperidol at 0.075 mg/kg or less does not reduce the motor capacity of animals and shows no side effects that might mask the target behavior43. In addition, haloperidol at 0.048 mg/kg virtually did not interfere with receptors other than D244. In Figure 8, blue density plots show the distribution of IRTs for rats in the three last sessions of the saline condition and salmon-colored density plots show the distribution of IRTs for the same subjects in the last three sessions of the haloperidol condition. Embedded bar plots depict comparisons between response rates (top) and between reward rates (bottom) within the same time frame of both conditions (color code: blue = saline, salmon = haloperidol).
Figure 8: Effect of haloperidol on DRL performance. Each panel shows a comparison between performance on the last 3 sessions in the saline administration stage (blue) and the haloperidol administration stage (salmon). The primary plots show the IRTs density distributions for individual subjects (Rat 2 died due to causes unrelated to the study) and averaged data (bottom right panel). Embedded plots display comparisons of response rates (A) and reward rates (B) in both stages with the same color code as the one used for density plots. Data was extracted from a recent unpublished study. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
As it can be seen in blue density plots, subjects display individual differences regarding the emission of burst responses. While rats 1 and 3 barely produce burst responses, a substantial proportion of rats' 4, 5, and 6 IRTs distribution was constituted by burst responses. The embedded bar plots show that haloperidol reduced overall response rate for three of five subjects, specifically for those subjects with a high proportion of burst responses. This illustrates that haloperidol mainly affects the response rate of those responses with very short IRTs, what can be corroborated with the pink density plots. Also, bar plots show that reward rate decreased for four out of five subjects. In average haloperidol administration slightly decreased both response and reward rates (see right bottom panel), which have been reported in other studies with rats45 and nonhuman primates46 using different target times (but see a study by Britton and Koob47 in which reward rate increased with the same dose). If one only considers global performance measures, this result may seem paradoxical given that this protocol is explicitly designed to prize low response rates (as its name implies). This result instantiates that a low rate of responding is not sufficient to yield an optimal exploitation of available rewards in this task. Examining the timed responses distribution in the density plots may shed light on the nature of this finding. While the peaks of the timed distributions did not systematically shift to either side with the administration of haloperidol, the spread increased drastically. This may reflect a disruption of temporal estimation, which has been previously reported using other procedures48.
The expected result was a decrease in impulsivity. Haloperidol is a high-affinity selective dopamine D2 receptor antagonist that acts mainly in the postsynaptic dopamine receptor. As mentioned above, dopaminergic system plays an important role in impulsive behavior. For instance, D2 receptor ligand binding in the NAc has been reported to predict increased impulsivity49. Also, dopamine NAc depletion decreases the frequency of premature responses in other protocols that measure the response inhibition component of impulsivity50. A possible interpretation of the observed results would be that the dose of haloperidol used was not sufficient to decrease substantially inhibition-related impulsivity while disrupting time estimation, causing disorganized responding and reward loss. This highlights the need for a more detailed analysis of IRTs to provide a more thorough interpretation of data, instead of just employing global measures as earlier reports have done.
Concerning feature-negative discrimination, Figure 9 shows the typical performance of a group of subjects in this protocol through 16 sessions. As is evidenced in the figure, responding in the A+ trials and in the AX- do not differ substantially in early sessions. After a few sessions, however, rats responded differentially in both types of trials, which reveal that the stimulus X is counteracting the response tendency controlled by the A stimulus. Note that subjects withhold magazine approach responses without any punishment in AX- trials. Importantly, subjects show quite robust individual differences in both responding to A+ trials and AX- trials, as shown by the error bars. This is further instantiated in Figure 10, which depicts individual examples of extreme cases with regards to the degree of response inhibition displayed in this protocol.
Figure 9: Longitudinal progression of performance on a feature-negative discrimination protocol for a group of rats. Points represent mean conditioned response (magazine approach) durations for six rats in each of 16 sessions. Black points identify responding in A+ trials and grey points identify responding in AX- trials. Error bars represent 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 10: Comparison of response durations in A+ and AX- trials for two extreme individuals on a feature-negative discrimination protocol. Upper panel shows the performance of a high-impulsivity individual (Rat I1) and the bottom panel shows the performance of a low-impulsivity subject (Rat I6). Histograms represent distributions of response durations in the four last sessions; green identifies responding in the A+ trials and purple identifies responding in the AX- trials. Here, impulsivity is indicated by the overlap between distributions. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
The present article provided a description of a miscellaneous variety of protocols for screening impulsivity in rats. It is argued that these particular protocols are favored for their ease of programming and data analysis and require fewer operating and stimulus devices than other available alternatives. There are several crucial steps for the effective implementation of these protocols, such as (1) yielding a research question, (2) selecting an appropriate study design, (3) programming the selected protocol, (4) conducting the study, (5) collecting the data, (6) analyzing the data, and (7) interpreting the data. Adequately developing the research question helps narrowing the range of possible ways to approach the topic. A focused research question will likely lead to an appropriate study design, which will inform researchers about the selected topic. One of the cardinal features of these protocols is that they are largely automated. In order to produce a flawless program for operating the conditioning chamber and collecting the data automatically, a thorough code needs to be written. If well conducted (daily run, at the same hour, by the same experimenters, and accounting for the major confounding factors), these protocols could yield to fair volumes of data that can be interpreted at a large range of resolutions; for example, in a molecular fashion (response by response), in a trial by trial fashion, within sessions blocks, across sessions, etc.
The protocols presented in this article have been validated elsewhere. For example, using the concurrent-chains version of the intertemporal choice procedure, Orduña37 found strong evidence that a rat model for ADHD performed poorly compared with animals in a control group (see Figure 6). Although this result can be taken as strong evidence in support of the validity of this animal model, there could be, at least, an alternative explanation. Animals could prefer the SS alternative not because of a strong delay discounting but rather due to a poor sensitivity to the magnitude of reward. Subsequent experiments by this author ruled out this possibility (Experiment 2) and ultimately confirmed that differences in performance between strains are indeed due to differences in delay discounting (Experiment 3). This was elegantly accomplished by using the concurrent chains to evaluate sensitivity to reward magnitude and delay discounting in isolation; that is, assessing preference between varying amounts of rewards maintaining the duration of delay constant and vice versa. As it may be recalled, delay discounting is assumed to be directly relevant to impulsivity.
The delay discounting feature of impulsivity has been extensively studied with protocols that manipulate delays or reward amounts in either within or between-sessions fashion51,52. Such a practice allows the researcher to mathematically characterize the decay in reward value as a function of the delay. However, using several values of the delay or the magnitude is not necessary for assessing the degree in which a delayed outcome affects the preference for that outcome, as the study of Orduña37 showed that differences in performance in a delay discounting protocol are due to differences in sensitivity to delay. Moreover, using a single delay value would be desirable if one aims to apply multiple protocols or evaluating subjects within a brief developmental stage. The present article presents the concurrent-chains schedule as a convenient alternative53, which is considerably straightforward among paradigms to assess delay-discounting associated impulsivity that is easy to program, to conduct, and to interpret.
DRL procedures have also been empirically validated. For example, van den Broek et al.54 selected impulsive and non-impulsive woman participants based on performance in a figure-matching task. These authors reported that impulsive participants tended to perform poorly in a DRL task compared with non-impulsive participants in several situations. Similarly, Orduña et al.31 found differences between SHR and Wistar rats in performance on a DRL protocol. However, differences were observed only in early sessions. As sessions passed, the strain differences vanished. This indicates that the protocol (or, again, at least the particular parameters employed) is only able to detect differences in learning rates rather than in steady states of these rat strains. It is important to note that a wide range of target times have been used in the DRL literature. However, it seems that different target times have been related to distinct psychiatric conditions; while shorter target times have been typically used to model impulse control disorders31,32,33, larger ones have been used to screen for depression55,56,57. That seems to support the idea that different processes impacting behavior under the constraints of shorter and longer target times33. That was the reason for selecting 10-second target times in the Representative Results section. In addition, larger target times need to be introduced progressively over a number of steps, which increases the duration of the protocol.
There are also studies that validate feature-negative discrimination procedures as protocols to assess impulsivity. For example, He et al.58 found that participants labeled as impulsive perform poorly in a transfer test (i.e., summation) for feature-negative discrimination protocol (but see another study by He et al.59). In another study, Bucci et al.60 assessed feature-negative discrimination performance by SHR and a control strain of rats. Although failing to observe overall differences in performance between strains, these authors found sex differences that mimic those found in humans. Namely, female SHRs showed an impaired performance in the task. This could be compared to clinical data with humans, where females diagnosed with ADHD show more extreme symptoms than males61. A converging line of evidence that validates feature-negative discrimination as a model of impulsivity comes from a study conducted by Meyer and Bucci40. These authors reported that performance in a feature-negative discrimination was impaired by lesions in the prefrontal cortex. This brain structure is assumed to play an important role on impulse control5 and, indeed, lesions in this structure have been documented to impair performance in other protocols to assess impulsivity62, which provides the feature-negative discrimination procedure with face validity. In spite of the fact that feature-negative discrimination protocol is not as widely used to test impulsivity as other procedures, it was included due to practical reasons, its face and construct validity, and because of the large body of empiric data and theoretical developments that have documented the mechanisms involved in the performance in this procedure63,64.
The feature-negative discrimination procedure has been a hallmark for inducing a learning phenomenon known as conditioned inhibition. In order to unambiguously demonstrate this phenomenon, it is widely believed that two complementary tests have to be jointly passed65 (although a number of authors have disputed the necessity and sufficiency of those tests66,67,68,69). In a summation test, the feature stimulus (X in the current notation) would decrease responding elicited by a conditioned stimulus other than that trained along with it (A). In a retardation test, the stimulus X would acquire conditioned responding slower than a control stimulus. However, these are tests for demonstrating that stimulus X is indeed inhibitory according to the theoretical characterization of conditioned inhibition. Tests for conditioned inhibition are not necessary for evaluating the learned capacity of an individual or group to withhold a prepotent approach response in presence of a cue associated with the omission of food.
The procedures described in this article may allow researchers to perform a battery of behavioral tests for impulsivity. As mentioned before, combining multiple impulsivity tests has been shown to synergize the predictive power of the protocols12, which would be useful on both theoretical and applied grounds. An additional advantage of assessing different manifestations of impulsivity within a single study is providing content validity (a special type of construct validity), under the assumption that impulsivity is a multifaceted phenomenon. However, caution must be exercised when sequentially testing impulsivity with more than one of the tasks presented here, as there have been documented problems associated with such a practice. For instance, carryover effects could occur, which means that performance in a task can be heavily influenced by learning in previous tasks; this type of effect could even arise within different conditions within the same task70. Another inconvenient consequence of applying more than two tasks in the same subjects is that, given the life cycle of rodents, tests sometimes would be implemented at different developmental stages71. There are some actions to minimize such outcomes, such as counterbalancing the sequence of application of tasks (which anyway would be troublesome for correlational studies, as each particular sequence could not be grouped with the others for analyses) or finding protocols of short duration.
While fairly useful and convenient, the protocols presented in this article have some limitations. For example, several studies have reported weak non-significant correlations between measures from different categories (or even within the same category72) of protocols to assess impulsivity73,74,75,76. Such finding challenges the concurrent validity of the protocols, prompting some authors to suppose that each category of protocols measure, in fact, independent factors contributing to impulsive behavior5,10,77,78. However, others have stressed in the shared features of the protocols and proposed unifying frameworks to account for different forms of impulsive behavior4,6,20,79,80. There could be room for doubt in studies showing the absence of correlation and not including intra-class correlations reports or other tests for quantifying the psychometric properties of their measures14,15. Although the prevailing belief is that impulsivity is multifaceted, more research with an acknowledgeable statistical power is needed to quantify at what degree. Another known limitation is that processes unrelated to impulsivity may contribute to performance in these protocols81. For example, as described above, in the DRL procedure performance is determined not only by response inhibition capability, but also by time estimation. Other authors have suggested that motivational and motoric factors may as well contribute to performance in this protocol33,82. Fortunately, ancillary methods have been devised to rule out some of these factors32,73. Yet another limitation is that laboratory protocols for nonhuman animals are not exact analogues of those typically used with humans11; thus, their validity as translational research methods is moot. Studies that assessed the performance of humans in versions of the protocols more closely related to those typically used with nonhumans lead to similar results83.
Only three protocols were presented. However, a handful of alternative options are available. Examples of these alternatives are the 5-CSRTT (for which there is also a video-article available84), the go/no-go task85, the stop-signal task86, and the sign-tracking paradigm87. The 5-CSRTT has been also validated as a model for ADHD, but it is devised to focus on the inattention feature of this condition (although response inhibition also contributes to performance). This task also requires a customized panel inserted in one of the side walls of the conditioning chamber, requiring at least 5 input and 5 output additional devices (which increases costs). Performance on go/no-go and the stop-signal tasks have shown to be related to several psychiatric conditions involving impulsivity88,89,90,91,92. These tasks are fairly similar to feature-negative discrimination protocols, but with the additional aspect that rewards are delivered depending on subjects' performance20. Such peculiarity implies slightly more complex coding for automatic operation and data collecting and analysis. Lastly, the sign-tracking paradigm has also been theoretically and empirically related to impulsivity79. However, for optimal results, it requires the attachment of a light-emitting devise to the levers76, which can also increase costs.
The protocols described here could be considered to be promising as performance in these protocols is sensitive to meaningful biological manipulations, such as selective breeding (see Figure 6), pharmacological interventions (see Figure 8), and brain lesions40. However, a review of the literature often reveals mixed results regarding the direction of the effects. Future applications of these methods should systematically study which parameters yield to stronger effects by adopting a parametrical approach. This would enable researchers to select parameters of a given protocol depending on the study design. For example, correlational studies require high reliable inter-individual variation for an appropriate statistical power, while conversely experimental studies benefit from measures with low intra-subject variance but are sensitive to situational manipulations14. A research agenda should consider these matters in order to efficiently contribute to the knowledge about impulsivity.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
We would like to thank Florencia Mata, María Elena Chávez, Miguel Burgos, and Alejandro Tapia for providing technical assistance. We also wish to thank Sarah Gordon Frances for her useful comments on a previous draft of this article and Vladimir Orduña for kindly providing raw data from a published paper. Thanks to Claudio Nallen for creating the diagram in Figure 1. We are grateful to the Dirección de Investigación of the Universidad Iberoamericana Ciudad de México for funding proofreading/editing services and the video producing expenses.
25 Pin Cables | Med Associates | SG-213F | Connect smart control cards to smart control panels |
40 Pin Ribbon Cable | Med Associates | DIG-700C | Connects the computer with the interface cabinet |
Computer | Dell Computer Company | T8P8T-7G8MR-4YPQV-96C2F-7THHB | For controlling and monitoring protocols’ processes |
Conductor Cables | Med Associates | SG-210CP-8 | Provide power to the smart control panels via the rack mount power supply |
Food dispenser with pedestal | Med Associates | ENV-203M-45 (12937) | Silently provides 45 mg food pellets |
Head-Entry Detector | Med Associates | ENV-254-CB | Uses an infrared photo-beam to detect head entries into the food receptacle |
House Light | Med Associates | ENV-215M | For providing diffuse illumination inside the chamber |
Interface Cabinet | Med Associates | SG-6080D | Pod that can hold up to eight smart control cards |
Med-PC IV Software | Med Associates | SOF-735 | Translate codes into commands for operating outputs and recording/storing input information |
Multiple tone generator | Med Associates | ENV-223 (597) | For controlling the frequency of the tones |
Panel fillers | Med Associates | ENV-007-FP | For filling modular walls when devices are not used |
Pellet Receptacle | Med Associates | ENV-200R2M | Receives and holds food pellets delivered by the dispenser |
Rack Mount Power Supply | Med Associates | DIG-700F | Provides power to the interface cabinet |
Retractable Lever | Med Associates | ENV-112CM (10455) | Detects lever-pressing responses; projects into the chamber or retracts as needed |
Smart Control Cards | Med Associates | DIG-716 | Controls up to eight inputs and four outputs of a conditioning chamber |
Smart Control Panels | Med Associates | SG-716 (3341) | Connect smart cards to the devices within the conditioning chambers |
Speaker | Med Associates | ENV-224AM | For providing tones inside the chamber |
Standard Modular Chambers for Rat | Med Associates | ENV-008 | Made of aluminum channels designed to hold modular devices |
Standard sound-, light-, and temperature isolating shells | Med Associates | ENV-022MD | Serve to harbor each conditioning chamber |
Stimulus Light | Med Associates | ENV-221M | For providing a round focalized light stimulus |
Three Pin Cables | Med Associates | SG-216A-2 | Connects smart control panel with each of the input and output devices in the conditioning chambers |