Summary

Three Laboratory Procedures for Assessing Different Manifestations of Impulsivity in Rats

Published: March 17, 2019
doi:

Summary

We present three protocols that assess different forms of impulsivity in rats and other small mammals. Intertemporal choice procedures evaluate the tendency to discount the value of delayed outcomes. Differential reinforcement of low rates and feature-negative discrimination evaluate response inhibition capacity with and without punishment for inappropriate responses, respectively.

Abstract

The present article provides a guide for the conduction and analysis of three conditioning-based protocols to evaluate impulsivity in rats. Impulsivity is a meaningful concept because it is associated with psychiatric conditions in humans and with maladaptive behavior in non-human animals. It is believed that impulsivity is composed of separate factors. There are laboratory protocols devised to assess each of these factors using standardized automated equipment. Delay discounting is associated with the incapacity to be motivated by delayed outcomes. This factor is evaluated through intertemporal choice protocols, which consist of presenting the individual with a choice situation involving an immediate reward and a larger but delayed reward. Response inhibition deficit is associated with the incapacity to withhold prepotent responses. Differential reinforcement of low rates (DLR) and feature-negative discrimination protocols assess the response inhibition deficit factor of impulsivity. The former imposes a condition to a motivated individual in which most wait a minimum period of time for a response to be rewarded. The latter evaluates the capacity of individuals to refrain from food seeking responses when a signal of the absence of food is presented. The purpose of these protocols is to construct an objective quantitative measure of impulsivity, which serves to make cross-species comparisons, allowing the possibility of translational research. The advantages of these particular protocols include their easy set-up and application, which stems from the relatively small amount of equipment needed and the automated nature of these protocols.

Introduction

Impulsivity can be conceptualized as a behavioral dimension associated with maladaptive outcomes1. Despite the widespread use of this term, there is no universal consensus on its precise definition. In fact, several authors have defined impulsivity by giving examples of impulsive behaviors or their consequences, rather than delineating which distinctive aspects govern the phenomenon. For instance, impulsivity is assumed to involve an inability to wait, plan, inhibit prepotent behaviors, or an insensitivity to delayed outcomes2, and it has been considered a core vulnerability to addictive behavior3. Bari and Robbins4 have characterized impulsivity as the co-occurrence of strong impulses, being triggered by dispositional and situational variables, and dysfunctional inhibitory processes. A different definition was provided by Dalley and Robbins, who stated that impulsivity could be regarded as a predisposition to rapid, often premature, actions without appropriate insight5. Yet, another definition of impulsivity, proposed by Sosa and dos Santos6, is a behavior tendency that deviates an organism from maximizing available rewards due to the acquired control exerted over the organism's responding by stimuli incidentally related to those rewards.

Due to the behavioral processes related to impulsivity, its neurophysiological substrate involves structures in common with those of motivated behavior, decision making and reward valuing. This is supported by studies that show that structures of the cortico-striatal pathway (e.g., nucleus accumbens [NAc], prefrontal cortex [PFC], amygdala, and caudate putamen [CPU]), as well as the ascending monoaminergic neurotransmitter system, participate in the expression of impulsive behavior7. However, the neural substrate of impulsivity is more complex than that. Although NAc and PFC are involved in impulsive behavior, these structures are part of a more complex system, and also are composed by substructures that have different functions (for more detailed documentation, see Dalley and Robbins5).

Regardless of the controversies about its nature and biological substrate, this behavioral dimension is known to vary across individuals, in which case it can be considered as a trait, and within individuals, in which case it can be considered as a state8. Impulsivity has long been recognized as a feature of some psychiatric conditions, such as attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), substance abuse, and manic episodes9. There seems to be a high consensus that impulsivity is composed by multiple dissociable factors, including unwillingness to wait (i.e., delay discounting), incapacity to refrain prepotent responses (i.e., inhibitory deficit), difficulty to focus on relevant information (i.e., inattention), and a tendency to engage in risky situations (i.e., sensation seeking)5,10,11. Each of these factors can be assessed through special behavioral tasks, which are usually assigned to two broad categories: choice and response inhibition (these may have different labels between each authors' taxonomies). Some important features of such behavioral tasks are that they could be applied across several animal species2 and that they allow studying impulsivity in controlled laboratory conditions.

Modeling a behavioral dimension with laboratory non-human animals has a number of advantages including the possibility of measuring specific, operationalized behavioral tendencies, allowing the researchers to largely reduce confounding variables (e.g., contamination by past life events4) and to implement experimental manipulations such as chronic pharmacological administration, performing neurotoxic lesions, or genetic manipulations. Most of these protocols have analogue versions for humans, which make comparisons easy5. Importantly, using analogues of these laboratory protocols in humans is effective to aid diagnosis of psychiatric conditions, such as ADHD (especially when more than one protocol is applied12).

Like any other psychological measurement, laboratory protocols for assessing impulsivity must comply with particular criteria in order to achieving the goal of providing insight into the phenomenon under study. To be considered as an appropriate model of impulsive behavior a laboratory protocol should be reliable, and possess (at least, in some degree) face, construct, and/or predictive validity13. Reliability could imply either that an effect upon the measurement would replicate if a manipulation is conducted two or more times, or that the measurement is consistent over time or across different situations14,15. The former feature would be especially useful for experimental studies, while the latter would be so for correlational studies14. Face validity refers to the degree in which what is measured resembles the phenomenon that is supposed to be modeled, as to being, for example, affected by the same variables. Predictive validity refers to the ability of a measure to forecast future performance in protocols, which aim to measure the same or a related construct. Finally, construct validity refers to whether the protocol reproduces behavior that is theoretically sound regarding the process or processes assumed to be involved in the phenomenon under study. However, although these are highly desirable features, one should be cautious when stating that a protocol is valid purely based on these criteria16.

There are several protocols to measure impulsivity in laboratory settings. However, the present article presents only three such methods: intertemporal choice, differential reinforcement of low rates, and feature-negative discrimination. Intertemporal procedures aim to assess the delay discounting (i.e., the difficulty of delayed outcomes to control behavior) component of impulsivity. The basic rationale of this protocol is confronting subjects with two rewards that differ in both magnitude and delay17. One alternative provides a small immediate reward (termed smaller sooner, SS) and the other provides a larger but delayed reward (termed larger later, LL). The proportion of responses to the SS alternative can be used as an index of impulsivity18. In differential reinforcement of low rates procedures, the factor of impulsivity to be assessed is response inhibition (i.e., incapacity to withhold prepotent responses) when there is a negative punishment contingency upon inappropriate responding. The rationale of this protocol is introducing subjects to a situation in which the only way of obtaining rewards is to pause their responding19. Finally, feature-negative discrimination procedure evaluates response inhibition when there is no explicit punishment upon inappropriate responding. The rationale of this protocol (also known as Pavlovian conditioned inhibition or the A+/AX- procedure) is to evaluate subjects' ability to withhold unnecessary responses20.

These procedures stand out in comparison to others as having some convenient features. For example, the procedures presented here are suitable for being conducted in minimally equipped conditioning chambers (also known as 'the Skinner box'). Figure 1 shows a diagram of a typical conditioning chamber. Conditioning chambers are useful research instruments due to a number of advantages. They allow automated collection of a relatively large volume of data, maximizing the number of subjects assessed for unity of time and space21. Moreover, behavioral studies conducted in conditioning chambers require minimal researcher intervention, which reduces the time and effort invested by laboratory staff, unlike other available methods (e.g., non-automated T-mazes, set-shifting boxes)21. Minimizing researchers' intervention also help in reducing researchers' bias, decreasing effects of researchers' learning curve, and a reduction of handling-induced stress22. Typical conditioning chambers are fairly standardized to be used with medium sized rodents, such as rats (R. norvegicus), but can be employed to study other taxa, like similar-sized marsupials (e.g., D. albiventris, and L. crassicaudata23). There are also commercial conditioning chambers adapted for smaller (e.g., mice [M. musculus]) and larger (e.g., non-human primates) species. Setting up and conducting the protocols presented in this article require minimal programming skills and demand a quite low number of attainable input and output devices, unlike more sophisticated alternative methods (e.g., 5-choice serial reaction time task [5-CSRTT]24 and sign-tracking25).

Figure 1
Figure 1: Diagram of a conditioning chamber prototype. The main components of the conditioning chamber include: (1) left lever, (2) food receptacle (equipped with lateral infrared diodes to detect head entries), (3) focalized light, (4) speaker for tone emission (rear view), (5) house light (rear view), (6) food dispenser. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Protocol

The three protocols described in this section require the use of rats as subjects. Most laboratory rat strains are suitable; for example, Wistar, Long-Evans, Sprague-Dawley, etc. The Ethics Committee of the Universidad Iberoamericana, following the Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals (Institute of Laboratory Animal Resources, Commission on Life Sciences, National Research Council, 1996), approved the laboratory protocols to be described.

1. Animal Housing and Preparation

  1. Determine the number of rats that will be used. This will depend on several factors, such as the type of design selected, the statistical power desired/required, the costs of conducting the study, and the time available for conducting the study26.
  2. Label each rat’s tail with an indelible marker for identification purposes.
  3. House rats either individually or in groups (2-5) with water freely available.
  4. Restrict rats’ food intake in order to motivate them for the protocols. In the case of individually housed rats, a convenient method for food restriction is to reduce weight to 85% of free-feeding weigh (use only for adult rats)27. Maintain this goal weight by providing supplementary food after conducting the protocol. For group-housed rats, give access to food for 60 min daily after conducting the protocol27.
  5. House the conditioning chambers within the sound and light attenuating shells.

2. Preliminary Training

NOTE: Before starting any of these behavioral protocols rats need to become accustomed to the conditioning chambers and food pellets. It is also vital to train the responses with which the animals would operate in the protocol. The three protocols presented here use appetitive motivation to induce behavior indicative of impulsiveness, like most other available alternative tasks (with select exceptions28). Conventional food dispensers are well suited to deliver both commercial refined grain and sugar pellets but can even handle “raw” grain under certain circumstances29.

  1. Habituation
    1. After starting the food restriction regime, introduce the rats into the conditioning chambers without initiating any protocol for 30 min, in order to habituate exploring responses. Put 60 food pellets in the food receptacle at the beginning of the session in order to habituate food neophobia.
    2. Repeat daily until the rats consume all of the food pellets.
  2. Magazine training
    1. After the habituation stage, introduce the rats into the conditioning chambers for two additional 30 min daily sessions delivering a food pellet every 45 s. This helps the rats identify the source of food pellets.
  3. Lever-press training
    1. Use this only for intertemporal choice and DRL protocols.
    2. Project one (for DRL) or the two levers (for intertemporal choice) into the chambers and start a continuous reinforcement procedure, that is, deliver a food pellet for every lever press. This procedure is used concurrently with a free food pellet delivery every 45 s (i.e., an alternative FR1-FT45 s schedule of reinforcement30), as in the previous stage. Sessions can have durations of 30 min.
    3. Repeat daily after the rats earn 80 rewards for two consecutive days.
  4. Shaping by successive approximations
    1. Use this method in case the rats do not reach the criterion in four sessions.
    2. Open the isolating shell of the conditioning chamber and observe the rats’ behavior. Deliver a food pellet for every response that approximates the target response (i.e., lever pressing). Examples of these approximate responses are approaching, sniffing, or touching the lever.
    3. Once the rats consistently perform the approximate responses, stop delivering rewards upon them and start requiring a response that is closer to the target response. Repeat as necessary.

3. Programming Automated Protocols

NOTE: The used values (e.g., delays, reward amounts, number of trials, session durations, schedules’ values, time-out length, inter-trial interval span, threshold for forced trials, presence/absence of accompanying stimuli, stimuli durations) presented were arbitrarily selected. Readers may want to consult the literature for determining appropriate parameters and conditions for accomplishing their particular goals. Codes for conducting samples of the three protocols presented here in a MED-PC environment are provided in the repository that can be found in the following URL: https://github.com/SaavedraPablo/MED-PC-codes. Such codes can be freely downloaded and modified according to particular needs.

  1. Intertemporal choice
    1. Select the values for delay and magnitude of reward. For example, choices for the SS alternative deliver one food pellet immediately and choices for the LL alternative deliver five food pellets after a 20 s fixed delay.
    2. Select a finishing criterion. End sessions automatically after completion of some specified criterion. For example: end the session after 40 choice trials or after 50 min.
    3. Combine each alternative with a lever (left or right) within the conditioning chamber counterbalancing the laterality of the alternatives among subjects.
    4. Project both levers into the conditioning chambers and make alternatives SS and LL available upon the accomplishment of a variable interval schedule30. Once the first lever press after a certain interval has elapsed, this activates the associated alternative (delay included). Varying the duration of such an interval in a pseudo-random fashion prevents exclusive preference for a particular alternative.
    5. Retract both levers and activate the consequence associated with the SS or LL alternatives after accomplishment of a variable-interval schedule of reinforcement.
    6. Perform a time-out condition (signaled by a house-light blackout) after reward delivery. Adjust this duration of this condition to equate the average duration of inter-trial intervals for both alternatives. The next choice trial begins after the completion of the time-out. Figure 2 shows a diagram of events during two successive trials of an intertemporal choice procedure.
    7. Implement forced trials. If subjects select one alternative for two consecutive trials, the program will determine that the next trial will be a forced trial of the remaining alternative. That is, in the next trial both levers are available, but only one will operate. This ensures that the subjects experience the outcomes associated with both alternatives.
    8. Finish a daily session whenever a prespecified number of trials have been completed or whenever the maximum time has elapsed.

Figure 2
Figure 2: Diagram of input and output events in two consecutive trials of an intertemporal choice procedure. Diagram of a prototypical intertemporal choice procedure, illustrating an SS alternative choice and an LL alternative choice, in two consecutive trials. Each row depicts the timeline of occurrence of particular output or input events. Spikes in the SS timeline represent choices of the smaller-sooner alternative (upon the accomplishment of the variable-interval schedule). Spikes in the LL timeline represent choices of the larger later alternative (idem). Asterisks in the Rw timeline represent reward deliveries. Elevated plateaus in the OR timeline represent periods of opportunity to respond (they are usually signaled, and its duration varies depending on the time that the individual takes to accomplish to the specified criterion); TO stands for the timeout that begins after reward delivery and ends with the next trial; during this period both levers are retracted. Note that timeout durations vary depending on the type of trial (SS choice or LL choice) in order to keep inter-trial intervals equated. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. DLR
    1. Select the value of the minimum time after which responding will produce a reward. For example, 10 s.
    2. After the beginning of a session or after any lever-press response, start a countdown timer from the selected time value (e.g., 10 s) to zero. If subjects emit a response before the timer reaches the value of zero the timer resets, so that they must wait for a new opportunity to get a reward. If subjects emit a response after the timer reaches the value of zero, deliver a food pellet and reset the timer after 2 s (this allows the animal to consume the food). Figure 3 shows some possible responding patterns and their corresponding programmed consequences.
      NOTE: During the 2 s reward retrieving interval, responses are not counted, which may impact the proportion of burst responses in the rare cases when the rats eat the food quickly enough and happen to respond immediately afterwards or fail to detect the delivery of food. This could be ameliorated by using a cue signaling the 2 s reward retrieving interval31. However, previous research has shown that the amount of such responses is negligible even in the absence of signaling cues.
    3. Finish the session after a time and/or number of rewards criterion.

Figure 3
Figure 3: Diagram of a hypothetical response pattern and its programmed consequences in a DRL 15 s procedure. Spikes in the R timeline represent the timeline of responses spontaneously emitted by the subject. Asterisks in the Rw timeline represent the timeline of reward deliveries. Numbers below the Cl row represent a clock counting down from 15 s the amount of time remaining before the next opportunity to respond and earning a reward. Note that reward delivery only occurs if a response is given since a minimum time of 15 s has elapsed from the last response. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. Feature-negative discrimination
    1. Select stimuli durations, inter-trial interval durations, and finishing-criterion for sessions. For example, use 8 s durations for conditioned stimuli, variable 92 s inter-trial intervals and finishing criterion of 24 trials.
    2. Present pseudo-randomly two types of trials, A+ and AX-, at 50% of the times each; A and X represent stimulus types and plus and minus signs represent the presence or absence of food, respectively. A+ trials: turn on one of the focalized lights (stimulus A) for 8 s and then deliver two food pellets (+). AX- trials: turn on one of the focalized lights (either side) for 8 s and concurrently present a tone (stimulus X) but do not deliver food (-). Figure 4 shows a diagram of the programmed events for each type of trial.
    3. Finish the session after a time and/or number of trials criterion.

Figure 4
Figure 4: Diagram of the types of trial used in the feature-negative discrimination procedure. Elevations in the A timeline represent onsets of the excitatory stimulus. Elevations in the X timeline represent onsets on the inhibitory stimulus. Asterisks in the food timeline represent food delivery. (A) A+ trials include the presentation of the excitatory stimulus followed by food delivery. (B) AX- trials include the presentation of the excitatory stimulus in compound with the inhibitory stimulus without food delivery. Recall that trials must be interspersed randomly and set apart by relatively long inter-trial intervals for better results. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

4. Running the Protocols

  1. Conduct the protocol daily, at a standard time, always placing rats in the same operant chamber.
  2. Set up the protocols in the computer software. Make sure to appropriately label the output file with the subjects’ names, condition, and study.
  3. Clean the inner walls, ceiling, and grill floor of the operant chambers with an ethanol or chlorine solutions, in order to remove odors from previous sessions or previous studies.
  4. Check that all the crucial inputs and outputs work appropriately by manually activating and monitoring them by means of the computer.
  5. Check that the food dispenser holds enough food to deliver throughout the session.
  6. Move the housing cages with the rats inside close to the conditioning chambers.
  7. Open the housing cage and gently carry each rat to its corresponding conditioning chamber, closing the conditioning chambers and the isolating shells.
  8. Initiate the program and wait until the program is finished. If data is not saved automatically, save the output files of the session in the computer drive or elsewhere.
  9. Gently carry the rats back into their corresponding housing cages after the program is finished.
  10. Give complementary food to the rats according to the selected food restriction regime.

5. Data Collection and Analysis

NOTE: Codes for extracting and manipulating data from MED-PC output files (saved with the extension .txt) for each procedure are provided in the repository that can be found in the following URL: https://github.com/SaavedraPablo/MED-PC-to-R-codes.

  1. Intertemporal choice
    1. Record lever presses in the SS alternative and in the LL alternative.
    2. Divide the SS alternative responses by the total responses to obtain the proportion of impulsive responses. Alternatively, divide the SS alternative responses by the LL alternative responses to calculate the ratio of impulsive responses. Take the common logarithm of ratio data points in order to remove skewness from the distribution.

Figure 5
Figure 5: Histogram of IRTs for one rat in a single session on the DRL 10 s protocol. The distribution is bimodal, with one of the peaks at very short IRTs (burst responses) and the other localized near the time criterion of the protocol (timed responses). Note as well that there is an accumulation of a small number of responses to the right and relatively far from the timed distribution (attentional lapses). Data was extracted from the 9th session in the DRL protocol of Rat 6 in a recent unpublished study. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. DRL
    1. Set a counter variable in the program that increases with each unit of time from the beginning of the session.
    2. Record the value of the counter variable in a list of values for each one of the responses as they occur during the session. This will provide a cumulative record of responses; that is, the exact time in which each response occurred during the session.
    3. Get the cumulative record of responses and subtract each value, i, from the previous value, i – 1, in order to obtain the inter-response times (IRTs), which constitute the variable of interest.
    4. Plot a histogram of IRTs for one rat in one session with 1 s intervals in the X axis, in order to visually inspect the data. For a typical experienced subject, this should see as a bimodal distribution with a portion of the data amassed in the left and another portion of the data clustered near the selected temporal requirement of the DRL protocol. Figure 5 shows an example of typical performance in the DRL protocol for one rat in a single session.
    5. Classify types of IRTs. As stated above, the distribution of IRTs for a typical subject is bimodal. One possible interpretation of this shape is that it is composed of the mixture of (at least) two distributions reflecting separate processes32.
      1. Classify IERs indicating attentional lapses.
        1. Too long IRTs may be indicative of attentional lapses (i.e., periods on which rats were not engaged in the task)33. A useful practice for these means is to separate far-right outliers from the rest of the data32. For example, multiply the interquartile range of the rightward distribution by some arbitrary constant (e.g., 3) and add this number to the median of this distribution to determine a cutoff value that signals the boundary between attentional lapses and the rest of data32.
      2. Classify responses in either the leftward or the rightward distribution (once the outliers have been removed32).
        1. The leftward distribution or burst responses distribution is constituted by too short IRTs, which are interpreted as indicative of hyperactivity34 or as a lack of attention and/or response feedback35. On the other hand, IRTs on the rightward distribution or timed responses distribution are considered as indicative of responding in adjustment to the temporal constriction of the protocol32. Either use an arbitrary cutoff to classify the boundaries of leftwards and rightwards distributions31 or use mathematical modeling to do so32,33,36.
      3. Determine the parameters of the timed responses distribution.
        1. Pay close attention to the rightward distribution in an experienced animal, which usually takes most of the IRTs and is considered as the most important part of the data set.
        2. Two parameters of interest are the localization of its peak and its spread. The former gives an index of the capacity to inhibit premature responses; shifts to the left of the time criterion may be interpreted as indicative of impulsivity37. The latter is indicative of temporal estimation; the narrower the distribution, the greater the timing accuracy32,40,43. Estimate these parameters through simple descriptive statistics or by more sophisticated mathematical modeling40,43,33.
        3. For a useful guide to fitting DRL data to the theoretical distribution proposed by Sanabria and Killeen33, see the supplementary material provided by these authors.
      4. Obtain a global efficiency measure.
        1. If the finishing criterion of the session is temporal (i.e., session duration will be constant) divide the number of earned rewards by the number of responses emitted, for obtaining a measure of efficiency. If the finishing criterion is a specific number of rewards calculate reward rate, which is number of rewards divided by session duration. Note that these global measures say little about how animals are obtaining or losing the rewards in the protocol and must be used only as a rough guide.
  2. Feature-negative discrimination
    1. Record the frequency or the duration of responses during A+ and AX- trials. The primary measure of conditioned responding may be the mean response frequency38, the mean response duration39, or the percentage of trials with at least one response.
    2. After choosing the preferred conditioned responding measure, subtract the value of responding during A+ trials minus responding during AX- trials for each subject in a particular session. This will constitute a negative index of impulsivity40; that is, the less the difference between both values, the greater the impulsivity.
      NOTE: The data from this task lend quite well to analyses based on measures from signal detection theory41,42, which can be used to supplement simple subtraction measures.

Representative Results

The three protocols described in this article may be each conducted alone or in conjunction with other procedures; this will depend on the research question, which in turn will determine the study design. Some examples of study designs that are compatible with these protocols are: (1) time series studies, which aim to describe longitudinal changes in performance; (2) quantification of individual variability, which aims to determine the reliability of the measures; (3) cross-sectional correlation studies, which aims to evaluate whether performance in one protocol can be used to predict performance on another protocol conducted afterwards; (4) longitudinal correlation studies, which aim to ascertain whether performance in one protocol can be used to predict performance on another protocol conducted concurrently; (5) non-experimental group comparisons, which aim to assess whether two or more samples from different populations differ with regards to impulsive performance; (6) pretest-posttest comparisons, which aim to determine whether an intervention (e.g., behavioral, pharmacological, surgical) is effective in altering (e.g., increase, decrease, stabilize) impulsive performance; (7) experimental simple group comparisons, which aim to evaluate whether an intervention if effective in altering impulsive performance but pretest measuring is not available (e.g., in interventions made in early stages of development intended to impact in adult performance). This list is not intended to be exhaustive and combinations of study designs are possible and encouraged.

As stated above, the intertemporal choice procedure is designed to assess the delay-discounting component of impulsivity. The remaining two protocols are supposed to examine inhibitory capacity, which is assumed to be one of the core components of impulsivity. DRL protocols evaluate response inhibition when inappropriate responding is explicitly punished by reward omission. On the other hand, feature-negative discrimination assesses response inhibition when there is no nominal punishment contingency for inappropriate responses. Next, some representative results of one of each protocol from the present laboratory or elsewhere are described.

Figure 6 shows a comparison of performance in an intertemporal choice procedure from a sample of spontaneously hypertensive rats (SHR) and Wistar rats. The former is a widely accepted rat strain model of ADHD, while the latter is a usual control strain. The SS alternative delivered a single food pellet after a 2 s fixed interval schedule and the LL alternative delivered four food pellets after a 28 s fixed interval schedule (recall that these alternatives were available upon accomplishment of an initial schedule of reinforcement; in this case a variable interval of 30 s). As depicted, the log ratio of lever response rate associated with the SS alternative is higher in SHR compared to Wistar rats. This can be interpreted as SHR presenting a preference for the immediate reward at the expense of a richer but delayed alternative, a sign of high delay-discounting related impulsivity.

Figure 6
Figure 6: Comparison of preference for Alternative SS in an intertemporal choice procedure for SHR and Wistar Rats. The Y axis displays the log-transformed SS/LL ratios. Boxplots are constituted by data from the average of the last five sessions performance for a group of eight SHR and a group of eight Wistar Rats. Data was adapted from the study conducted by Orduña37 (Figure 2 and Figure 3) with the author's permission. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Regarding performance on DRL protocols, Figure 7 shows longitudinal data of a single rat with a 10 s temporal restraint on responding. As it can be seen, during the first sessions the rat emits a high proportion of burst responses but there is a decrease on further sessions. It also may be seen that in earlier sessions there are few responses near the temporal criterion of the protocol. However, as the animal acquires experience in the task, it eventually learns to respond around 10 s. This represents evidence of the role of learning in performance in this protocol. Note, however, that none of the IRTs lower than 10 s were rewarded; even in the 18th session, there is a great proportion of ineffective responses. Such a performance denotes an important quality of the protocol: at least with these parameters, the task is not easy to master, which is helpful in avoiding problems associated with ceiling effects.

Figure 7
Figure 7: Longitudinal progression of performance on a DRL protocol for one rat. Each of the stacked plots displays the estimate of the probability density distribution of IRTs for one subject (Rat 2) along 18 sessions. Data was extracted from a recent unpublished study. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

An example of a pharmacological effect on DRL performance is shown in Figure 8. After reaching a steady performance in a DRL procedure with a target time of 10 s, five female rats received a 1 mL/kg subcutaneous injection of saline and were tested in the same procedure 30 min later for eight consecutive days. Then, saline was replaced with an equal volume of 0.05 mg/kg haloperidol and performance was tested for six more sessions. This aimed at testing whether impulsive performance in this procedure was decreased via D2 receptors antagonism. The dose was selected because it is known that haloperidol at 0.075 mg/kg or less does not reduce the motor capacity of animals and shows no side effects that might mask the target behavior43. In addition, haloperidol at 0.048 mg/kg virtually did not interfere with receptors other than D244. In Figure 8, blue density plots show the distribution of IRTs for rats in the three last sessions of the saline condition and salmon-colored density plots show the distribution of IRTs for the same subjects in the last three sessions of the haloperidol condition. Embedded bar plots depict comparisons between response rates (top) and between reward rates (bottom) within the same time frame of both conditions (color code: blue = saline, salmon = haloperidol).

Figure 8
Figure 8: Effect of haloperidol on DRL performance. Each panel shows a comparison between performance on the last 3 sessions in the saline administration stage (blue) and the haloperidol administration stage (salmon). The primary plots show the IRTs density distributions for individual subjects (Rat 2 died due to causes unrelated to the study) and averaged data (bottom right panel). Embedded plots display comparisons of response rates (A) and reward rates (B) in both stages with the same color code as the one used for density plots. Data was extracted from a recent unpublished study. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

As it can be seen in blue density plots, subjects display individual differences regarding the emission of burst responses. While rats 1 and 3 barely produce burst responses, a substantial proportion of rats' 4, 5, and 6 IRTs distribution was constituted by burst responses. The embedded bar plots show that haloperidol reduced overall response rate for three of five subjects, specifically for those subjects with a high proportion of burst responses. This illustrates that haloperidol mainly affects the response rate of those responses with very short IRTs, what can be corroborated with the pink density plots. Also, bar plots show that reward rate decreased for four out of five subjects. In average haloperidol administration slightly decreased both response and reward rates (see right bottom panel), which have been reported in other studies with rats45 and nonhuman primates46 using different target times (but see a study by Britton and Koob47 in which reward rate increased with the same dose). If one only considers global performance measures, this result may seem paradoxical given that this protocol is explicitly designed to prize low response rates (as its name implies). This result instantiates that a low rate of responding is not sufficient to yield an optimal exploitation of available rewards in this task. Examining the timed responses distribution in the density plots may shed light on the nature of this finding. While the peaks of the timed distributions did not systematically shift to either side with the administration of haloperidol, the spread increased drastically. This may reflect a disruption of temporal estimation, which has been previously reported using other procedures48.

The expected result was a decrease in impulsivity. Haloperidol is a high-affinity selective dopamine D2 receptor antagonist that acts mainly in the postsynaptic dopamine receptor. As mentioned above, dopaminergic system plays an important role in impulsive behavior. For instance, D2 receptor ligand binding in the NAc has been reported to predict increased impulsivity49. Also, dopamine NAc depletion decreases the frequency of premature responses in other protocols that measure the response inhibition component of impulsivity50. A possible interpretation of the observed results would be that the dose of haloperidol used was not sufficient to decrease substantially inhibition-related impulsivity while disrupting time estimation, causing disorganized responding and reward loss. This highlights the need for a more detailed analysis of IRTs to provide a more thorough interpretation of data, instead of just employing global measures as earlier reports have done.

Concerning feature-negative discrimination, Figure 9 shows the typical performance of a group of subjects in this protocol through 16 sessions. As is evidenced in the figure, responding in the A+ trials and in the AX- do not differ substantially in early sessions. After a few sessions, however, rats responded differentially in both types of trials, which reveal that the stimulus X is counteracting the response tendency controlled by the A stimulus. Note that subjects withhold magazine approach responses without any punishment in AX- trials. Importantly, subjects show quite robust individual differences in both responding to A+ trials and AX- trials, as shown by the error bars. This is further instantiated in Figure 10, which depicts individual examples of extreme cases with regards to the degree of response inhibition displayed in this protocol.

Figure 9
Figure 9: Longitudinal progression of performance on a feature-negative discrimination protocol for a group of rats. Points represent mean conditioned response (magazine approach) durations for six rats in each of 16 sessions. Black points identify responding in A+ trials and grey points identify responding in AX- trials. Error bars represent 95% bootstrapped confidence intervals. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 10
Figure 10: Comparison of response durations in A+ and AX- trials for two extreme individuals on a feature-negative discrimination protocol. Upper panel shows the performance of a high-impulsivity individual (Rat I1) and the bottom panel shows the performance of a low-impulsivity subject (Rat I6). Histograms represent distributions of response durations in the four last sessions; green identifies responding in the A+ trials and purple identifies responding in the AX- trials. Here, impulsivity is indicated by the overlap between distributions. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Discussion

The present article provided a description of a miscellaneous variety of protocols for screening impulsivity in rats. It is argued that these particular protocols are favored for their ease of programming and data analysis and require fewer operating and stimulus devices than other available alternatives. There are several crucial steps for the effective implementation of these protocols, such as (1) yielding a research question, (2) selecting an appropriate study design, (3) programming the selected protocol, (4) conducting the study, (5) collecting the data, (6) analyzing the data, and (7) interpreting the data. Adequately developing the research question helps narrowing the range of possible ways to approach the topic. A focused research question will likely lead to an appropriate study design, which will inform researchers about the selected topic. One of the cardinal features of these protocols is that they are largely automated. In order to produce a flawless program for operating the conditioning chamber and collecting the data automatically, a thorough code needs to be written. If well conducted (daily run, at the same hour, by the same experimenters, and accounting for the major confounding factors), these protocols could yield to fair volumes of data that can be interpreted at a large range of resolutions; for example, in a molecular fashion (response by response), in a trial by trial fashion, within sessions blocks, across sessions, etc.

The protocols presented in this article have been validated elsewhere. For example, using the concurrent-chains version of the intertemporal choice procedure, Orduña37 found strong evidence that a rat model for ADHD performed poorly compared with animals in a control group (see Figure 6). Although this result can be taken as strong evidence in support of the validity of this animal model, there could be, at least, an alternative explanation. Animals could prefer the SS alternative not because of a strong delay discounting but rather due to a poor sensitivity to the magnitude of reward. Subsequent experiments by this author ruled out this possibility (Experiment 2) and ultimately confirmed that differences in performance between strains are indeed due to differences in delay discounting (Experiment 3). This was elegantly accomplished by using the concurrent chains to evaluate sensitivity to reward magnitude and delay discounting in isolation; that is, assessing preference between varying amounts of rewards maintaining the duration of delay constant and vice versa. As it may be recalled, delay discounting is assumed to be directly relevant to impulsivity.

The delay discounting feature of impulsivity has been extensively studied with protocols that manipulate delays or reward amounts in either within or between-sessions fashion51,52. Such a practice allows the researcher to mathematically characterize the decay in reward value as a function of the delay. However, using several values of the delay or the magnitude is not necessary for assessing the degree in which a delayed outcome affects the preference for that outcome, as the study of Orduña37 showed that differences in performance in a delay discounting protocol are due to differences in sensitivity to delay. Moreover, using a single delay value would be desirable if one aims to apply multiple protocols or evaluating subjects within a brief developmental stage. The present article presents the concurrent-chains schedule as a convenient alternative53, which is considerably straightforward among paradigms to assess delay-discounting associated impulsivity that is easy to program, to conduct, and to interpret.

DRL procedures have also been empirically validated. For example, van den Broek et al.54 selected impulsive and non-impulsive woman participants based on performance in a figure-matching task. These authors reported that impulsive participants tended to perform poorly in a DRL task compared with non-impulsive participants in several situations. Similarly, Orduña et al.31 found differences between SHR and Wistar rats in performance on a DRL protocol. However, differences were observed only in early sessions. As sessions passed, the strain differences vanished. This indicates that the protocol (or, again, at least the particular parameters employed) is only able to detect differences in learning rates rather than in steady states of these rat strains. It is important to note that a wide range of target times have been used in the DRL literature. However, it seems that different target times have been related to distinct psychiatric conditions; while shorter target times have been typically used to model impulse control disorders31,32,33, larger ones have been used to screen for depression55,56,57. That seems to support the idea that different processes impacting behavior under the constraints of shorter and longer target times33. That was the reason for selecting 10-second target times in the Representative Results section. In addition, larger target times need to be introduced progressively over a number of steps, which increases the duration of the protocol.

There are also studies that validate feature-negative discrimination procedures as protocols to assess impulsivity. For example, He et al.58 found that participants labeled as impulsive perform poorly in a transfer test (i.e., summation) for feature-negative discrimination protocol (but see another study by He et al.59). In another study, Bucci et al.60 assessed feature-negative discrimination performance by SHR and a control strain of rats. Although failing to observe overall differences in performance between strains, these authors found sex differences that mimic those found in humans. Namely, female SHRs showed an impaired performance in the task. This could be compared to clinical data with humans, where females diagnosed with ADHD show more extreme symptoms than males61. A converging line of evidence that validates feature-negative discrimination as a model of impulsivity comes from a study conducted by Meyer and Bucci40. These authors reported that performance in a feature-negative discrimination was impaired by lesions in the prefrontal cortex. This brain structure is assumed to play an important role on impulse control5 and, indeed, lesions in this structure have been documented to impair performance in other protocols to assess impulsivity62, which provides the feature-negative discrimination procedure with face validity. In spite of the fact that feature-negative discrimination protocol is not as widely used to test impulsivity as other procedures, it was included due to practical reasons, its face and construct validity, and because of the large body of empiric data and theoretical developments that have documented the mechanisms involved in the performance in this procedure63,64.

The feature-negative discrimination procedure has been a hallmark for inducing a learning phenomenon known as conditioned inhibition. In order to unambiguously demonstrate this phenomenon, it is widely believed that two complementary tests have to be jointly passed65 (although a number of authors have disputed the necessity and sufficiency of those tests66,67,68,69). In a summation test, the feature stimulus (X in the current notation) would decrease responding elicited by a conditioned stimulus other than that trained along with it (A). In a retardation test, the stimulus X would acquire conditioned responding slower than a control stimulus. However, these are tests for demonstrating that stimulus X is indeed inhibitory according to the theoretical characterization of conditioned inhibition. Tests for conditioned inhibition are not necessary for evaluating the learned capacity of an individual or group to withhold a prepotent approach response in presence of a cue associated with the omission of food.

The procedures described in this article may allow researchers to perform a battery of behavioral tests for impulsivity. As mentioned before, combining multiple impulsivity tests has been shown to synergize the predictive power of the protocols12, which would be useful on both theoretical and applied grounds. An additional advantage of assessing different manifestations of impulsivity within a single study is providing content validity (a special type of construct validity), under the assumption that impulsivity is a multifaceted phenomenon. However, caution must be exercised when sequentially testing impulsivity with more than one of the tasks presented here, as there have been documented problems associated with such a practice. For instance, carryover effects could occur, which means that performance in a task can be heavily influenced by learning in previous tasks; this type of effect could even arise within different conditions within the same task70. Another inconvenient consequence of applying more than two tasks in the same subjects is that, given the life cycle of rodents, tests sometimes would be implemented at different developmental stages71. There are some actions to minimize such outcomes, such as counterbalancing the sequence of application of tasks (which anyway would be troublesome for correlational studies, as each particular sequence could not be grouped with the others for analyses) or finding protocols of short duration.

While fairly useful and convenient, the protocols presented in this article have some limitations. For example, several studies have reported weak non-significant correlations between measures from different categories (or even within the same category72) of protocols to assess impulsivity73,74,75,76. Such finding challenges the concurrent validity of the protocols, prompting some authors to suppose that each category of protocols measure, in fact, independent factors contributing to impulsive behavior5,10,77,78. However, others have stressed in the shared features of the protocols and proposed unifying frameworks to account for different forms of impulsive behavior4,6,20,79,80. There could be room for doubt in studies showing the absence of correlation and not including intra-class correlations reports or other tests for quantifying the psychometric properties of their measures14,15. Although the prevailing belief is that impulsivity is multifaceted, more research with an acknowledgeable statistical power is needed to quantify at what degree. Another known limitation is that processes unrelated to impulsivity may contribute to performance in these protocols81. For example, as described above, in the DRL procedure performance is determined not only by response inhibition capability, but also by time estimation. Other authors have suggested that motivational and motoric factors may as well contribute to performance in this protocol33,82. Fortunately, ancillary methods have been devised to rule out some of these factors32,73. Yet another limitation is that laboratory protocols for nonhuman animals are not exact analogues of those typically used with humans11; thus, their validity as translational research methods is moot. Studies that assessed the performance of humans in versions of the protocols more closely related to those typically used with nonhumans lead to similar results83.

Only three protocols were presented. However, a handful of alternative options are available. Examples of these alternatives are the 5-CSRTT (for which there is also a video-article available84), the go/no-go task85, the stop-signal task86, and the sign-tracking paradigm87. The 5-CSRTT has been also validated as a model for ADHD, but it is devised to focus on the inattention feature of this condition (although response inhibition also contributes to performance). This task also requires a customized panel inserted in one of the side walls of the conditioning chamber, requiring at least 5 input and 5 output additional devices (which increases costs). Performance on go/no-go and the stop-signal tasks have shown to be related to several psychiatric conditions involving impulsivity88,89,90,91,92. These tasks are fairly similar to feature-negative discrimination protocols, but with the additional aspect that rewards are delivered depending on subjects' performance20. Such peculiarity implies slightly more complex coding for automatic operation and data collecting and analysis. Lastly, the sign-tracking paradigm has also been theoretically and empirically related to impulsivity79. However, for optimal results, it requires the attachment of a light-emitting devise to the levers76, which can also increase costs.

The protocols described here could be considered to be promising as performance in these protocols is sensitive to meaningful biological manipulations, such as selective breeding (see Figure 6), pharmacological interventions (see Figure 8), and brain lesions40. However, a review of the literature often reveals mixed results regarding the direction of the effects. Future applications of these methods should systematically study which parameters yield to stronger effects by adopting a parametrical approach. This would enable researchers to select parameters of a given protocol depending on the study design. For example, correlational studies require high reliable inter-individual variation for an appropriate statistical power, while conversely experimental studies benefit from measures with low intra-subject variance but are sensitive to situational manipulations14. A research agenda should consider these matters in order to efficiently contribute to the knowledge about impulsivity.

Declarações

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Florencia Mata, María Elena Chávez, Miguel Burgos, and Alejandro Tapia for providing technical assistance. We also wish to thank Sarah Gordon Frances for her useful comments on a previous draft of this article and Vladimir Orduña for kindly providing raw data from a published paper. Thanks to Claudio Nallen for creating the diagram in Figure 1. We are grateful to the Dirección de Investigación of the Universidad Iberoamericana Ciudad de México for funding proofreading/editing services and the video producing expenses.

Materials

25 Pin Cables Med Associates SG-213F Connect smart control cards to smart control panels
40 Pin Ribbon Cable Med Associates DIG-700C Connects the computer with the interface cabinet
Computer Dell Computer Company T8P8T-7G8MR-4YPQV-96C2F-7THHB For controlling and monitoring protocols’ processes
Conductor Cables Med Associates SG-210CP-8 Provide power to the smart control panels via the rack mount power supply
Food dispenser with pedestal Med Associates ENV-203M-45 (12937) Silently provides 45 mg food pellets 
Head-Entry Detector Med Associates ENV-254-CB Uses an infrared photo-beam to detect head entries into the food receptacle
House Light Med Associates ENV-215M For providing  diffuse illumination inside the chamber  
Interface Cabinet Med Associates SG-6080D Pod that can hold up to eight smart control cards
Med-PC IV Software Med Associates SOF-735 Translate codes into commands for operating outputs and recording/storing input information
Multiple tone generator  Med Associates ENV-223 (597) For controlling the frequency of the tones
Panel fillers Med Associates ENV-007-FP For filling modular walls when devices are not used
Pellet Receptacle Med Associates ENV-200R2M Receives and holds food pellets delivered by the dispenser
Rack Mount Power Supply Med Associates DIG-700F Provides power to the interface cabinet
Retractable Lever Med Associates ENV-112CM (10455) Detects lever-pressing responses; projects into the chamber or retracts as needed
Smart Control Cards Med Associates DIG-716 Controls up to eight inputs and four outputs of a conditioning chamber 
Smart Control Panels Med Associates SG-716 (3341) Connect smart cards to the devices within the conditioning chambers
Speaker  Med Associates ENV-224AM For providing tones inside the chamber
Standard Modular Chambers for Rat Med Associates ENV-008 Made of aluminum channels designed to hold modular devices 
Standard sound-, light-, and temperature isolating shells Med Associates ENV-022MD Serve to harbor each conditioning chamber
Stimulus Light Med Associates ENV-221M For providing a round focalized light stimulus
Three Pin Cables Med Associates SG-216A-2 Connects smart control panel with each of the input and output devices in the conditioning chambers

Referências

  1. Loxton, N. J. The role of reward sensitivity and impulsivity in overeating and food addiction. Current Addiction Reports. 5 (2), 212-222 (2018).
  2. Richards, J. B., Gancarz, A. M., Hawk, L. W., Bardo, M. T., Fishbein, D. H., Milich, R. . Inhibitory control and drug abuse prevention. , (2011).
  3. Gullo, M. J., Loxton, N. J., Dawe, S. Impulsivity: Four ways five fectors are not basic to addiction. Addictive Behaviors. 39 (11), 1547-1556 (2014).
  4. Bari, A., Robbins, T. W. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Progress in Neurobiology. 108, 44-79 (2013).
  5. Dalley, J. W., Robbins, T. W. Fractionating impulsivity: neuropsychiatric implications. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 18 (3), 158-171 (2017).
  6. Sosa, R., dos Santos, C. V. Toward a unifying account of impulsivity and the development of self-control. Perspectives in Behavior Science. , 1-32 (2018).
  7. King, J. A., Tenney, J., Rossi, V., Colamussi, L., Burdick, S. Neural substrates underlying impulsivity. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1008 (1), 160-169 (2003).
  8. Stayer, R., Ferring, D., Schmitt, M. J. States and traits in psychological assessment. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 8 (2), 79-98 (1992).
  9. Moeller, F. G., Barratt, E. S., Dougherty, D. M., Schmitz, J. M., Swann, A. C. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. American Journal of Psychiatry. 158, 1783-1793 (2001).
  10. Evenden, J. L. Varieties of impulsivity. Psychopharmacology. 146 (4), 348-361 (1999).
  11. Winstanley, C. A. The utility of rat models of impulsivity in developing pharmacotherapies for impulse control disorders. British Journal of Pharmacology. 164 (4), 1301-1321 (2011).
  12. Solanto, M. V., et al. The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures of impulsivity in AD/HD: A supplement to the NIMH multimodal treatment study of AD/HD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 29 (3), 215-218 (2001).
  13. van der Staay, F. J. Animal models of behavioral dysfunctions: Basic concepts and classifications, and an evaluation strategy. Brain Research Reviews. 52, 131-159 (2006).
  14. Hedge, C., Powell, G., Summer, P. The reliability paradox: Why robust cognitive tasks do not produce reliable individual differences. Behavioral Research Methods. , 1-21 (2017).
  15. Nakagawa, S., Schielzeth, H. Repeatability for Gaussian and non-Gaussian data: A practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews. 85, 935-956 (2010).
  16. Sjoberg, E. Logical fallacies in animal model research. Behavior and Brain Functions. 13 (1), (2017).
  17. Rachlin, H. Self-control: Beyond commitment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 18 (01), 109 (1995).
  18. Logue, A. W. Research on self-control: An integrating framework. Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 11 (04), 665 (1988).
  19. Kramer, T. J., Rilling, M. Differential reinforcement of low rates: A selective critique. Psychological Bulletin. 74 (4), 225-254 (1970).
  20. Sosa, R., dos Santos, C. V. Conditioned inhibition and its relationship to impulsivity: Empirical and theoretical considerations. The Psychological Record. , (2018).
  21. Gallistel, C. R., Balci, F., Freestone, D., Kheifets, A., King, A. Automated, quantitative cognitive/behavioral screening of mice: For genetics, pharmacology, animal cognition and undergraduate instruction. Journal of Visualized Experiments. (84), (2014).
  22. Skinner, B. F. A case history in scientific method. American Psychologist. 11 (5), 221-233 (1956).
  23. Papini, M. R. Associative learning in the marsupials Didelphis albiventris and Lutreolina crassicaudata. Journal of Comparative Psychology. 102 (1), 21-27 (1988).
  24. Leonard, J. A. 5 choice serial reaction apparatus. Medical Research Council of Applied Psychology Research. , 326-359 (1959).
  25. Robinson, T. E., Flagel, S. B. Dissociating the Predictive and Incentive Motivational Properties of Reward-Related Cues Through the Study of Individual Differences. Biological Psychiatry. 65 (10), 869-873 (2009).
  26. Charan, J., Kantharia, N. D. How to calculate sample size in animal studies?. Journal of Pharmacology and Pharmacotherapeutics. 4 (4), 303-306 (2013).
  27. Toth, L. A., Gardiner, T. W. Food and water restriction protocols: Physiological and behavioral considerations. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science. 39 (6), 9-17 (2000).
  28. Deluty, M. Z. Self-control and impulsiveness involving aversive events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 4, 250-266 (1978).
  29. Cabrera, F., Robayo-Castro, B., Covarrubias, P. The ‘huautli’ alternative: Amaranth as reinforcer in operant procedures. Revista Mexicana de Análisis de la Conducta. 36, 71-92 (2010).
  30. Ferster, C. B., Skinner, B. F. . Schedules of reinforcement. , (1957).
  31. Orduña, V., Valencia-Torres, L., Bouzas, A. DRL performance of spontaneously hypertensive rats: Dissociation of timing and inhibition of responses. Behavioural Brain Research. 201 (1), 158-165 (2009).
  32. Freestone, D. M., Balci, F., Simen, P., Church, R. Optimal response rates in humans and animals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Cognition. 41 (1), 39-51 (2015).
  33. Sanabria, F., Killeen, P. R. Evidence for impulsivity in the Spontaneously Hypertensive Rat drawn from complementary response-withholding tasks. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 4 (1), 7 (2008).
  34. van den Bergh, F. S., et al. Spontaneously hypertensive rats do not predict symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 83, 11 (2006).
  35. Topping, J. S., Pickering, J. W. Effects of punishing different bands of IRTs on DRL responding. Psychological Reports. 31 (19-22), (1972).
  36. Richards, J. B., Sabol, K. E., Seiden, L. S. DRL interresponse-time distributions: quantification by peak deviation analysis. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 60 (2), 361-385 (1993).
  37. Orduña, V. Impulsivity and sensitivity to amount and delay of reinforcement in an animal model of ADHD. Behavioural Brain Research. 294, 62-71 (2015).
  38. Harmer, C. J., Phillips, G. D. Enhanced conditioned inhibition following repeated pretreatment with d -amphetamine. Psychopharmacology. 142 (2), 120-131 (1999).
  39. Lister, S., Pearce, J. M., Butcher, S. P., Collard, K. J., Foster, G. Acquisition of conditioned inhibition in rats is impaired by ablation of serotoninergic pathways. European Journal of Neuroscience. 8, 415-423 (1996).
  40. Meyer, H. C., Bucci, D. J. The contribution of medial prefrontal cortical regions to conditioned inhibition. Behavioral Neuroscience. 128 (6), 644-653 (2014).
  41. McNicol, D. . A primer of signal detection theory. , (1972).
  42. Carnero, S., Morís, J., Acebes, F., Loy, I. Percepción de la contingencia en ratas: Modulación fechneriana y metodología de la detección de señales. Revista Electrónica de Metodología Aplicada. 14 (2), (2009).
  43. López, H. H., Ettenberg, A. Dopamine antagonism attenuates the unconditioned incentive value of estrus female cues. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 68, 411-416 (2001).
  44. Schotte, A., Janssen, P. F. M., Megens, A. A. H. P., Leysen, J. E. Occupancy of central neurotransmitter receptors by risperidone, clozapine and haloperidol, measured ex vivo. Brain Research. 631 (2), 191-202 (1993).
  45. van Hest, A., van Haaren, F., van de Poll, N. Haloperidol, but not apomorphine, differentially affects low response rates of male and female wistar rats. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 29, 529-532 (1988).
  46. Finnegan, K. T., Ricaurte, G., Seiden, L. S., Schuster, C. R. Altered sensitivity to d-methylamphetamine, apomorphine, and haloperidol in rhesus monkeys depleted of caudate dopamine by repeated administration of d-methylamphetamine. Psychopharmacology. 77, 43-52 (1982).
  47. Britton, K. T., Koob, G. F. Effects of corticotropin releasing factor, desipramine and haloperidol on a DRL schedule of reinforcement. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 32, 967-970 (1989).
  48. Maricq, A. V., Church, R. The differential effects of haloperidol and metamphetamine on time estimation in the rat. Psychopharmacology. 79, 10-15 (1983).
  49. Dalley, J. W., et al. Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement. Science. 315, 1267-1270 (2007).
  50. Cole, B. J., Robbins, T. W. Effects of 6-hydroxydopamine lesions of the nucleus accumbens septi on performance of a 5-choice serial reaction time task in rats: Implications for theories of selective attention and arousal. Behavior and Brain Research. 33, 165-179 (1989).
  51. Reynolds, B., de Wit, H., Richards, J. B. Delay of gratification and delay discounting in rats. Behavioural Processes. 59 (3), 157-168 (2002).
  52. Evenden, J. L., Ryan, C. N. The pharmacology of impulsive behavior in rats: The effects of drugs on response choice with varying delays of reinforcement. Psychopharmacology. 128, 161-170 (1996).
  53. Autor, S. M., Hendry, D. P. . Conditioned reinforcement. , (1969).
  54. van den Broek, M. D., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E. Behaviour of ‘impulsive’ and ‘non-impulsive’ humans in a temporal differentiation schedule of reinforcement. Personality and Individual Differences. 8 (2), 233-239 (1987).
  55. McGuire, P. S., Seiden, L. S. The effects of tricyclicantidepressants on performance under a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rates schedule in rats. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 214 (3), 635-641 (1980).
  56. O’Donnell, J. M., Seiden, L. S. Differential-reinforcement-of-low-rates 72-second schedule: Selective effects of antidepressant drugs. Journal of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics. 224 (1), 80-88 (1983).
  57. Seiden, L. S., Dahms, J. L., Shaughnessy, R. A. Behavioral screen for antidepressants: The effects of drugs and electroconvulsive shock on performance under a differential-reinforcement-of-low-rates schedule. Psychopharmacology. 86, 55-60 (1985).
  58. He, Z., Cassaday, H. J., Howard, R. C., Khalifa, N., Bonardi, C. Impaired Pavlovian conditioned inhibition in offenders with personality disorders. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 64 (12), 2334-2351 (2011).
  59. He, Z., Cassaday, H. J., Bonardi, C., Bibi, P. A. Do personality traits predict individual differences in excitatory and inhibitory learning?. Frontiers in Psychology. 4, 1-12 (2013).
  60. Bucci, D. J., Hopkins, M. E., Keene, C. S., Sharma, M., Orr, L. E. Sex differences in learning and inhibition in spontaneously hypertensive rats. Behavioural Brain Research. 187 (1), 27-32 (2008).
  61. Gershon, J. A meta-analytic review of gender differences in ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders. 5, 143-154 (2012).
  62. Mobini, S., et al. Effects of lesions of the orbitofrontal cortex on sensitivity to delayed and probabilistic reinforcement. Psychopharmacology. 160 (3), 290-298 (2002).
  63. Bouton, M. E., Nelson, J. B. Context-specificity of target versus feature inhibition in a negative-feature discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. 20 (1), 51-65 (1994).
  64. Bouton, M. E., Nelson, J. B., Schmajuk, N., Holland, P. . Occasion setting: Associative learning and cognition in animals. , 69-112 (1998).
  65. Rescorla, R. A. Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin. 72 (2), 77-94 (1969).
  66. Miller, R. R., Matzel, L. D., Bower, G. H. . The psychology of learning and motivation. , (1988).
  67. Williams, D. A., Overmier, J. B., Lolordo, V. M. A reevaluation of Rescorla’s early dictums about conditioned inhibition. Psychological Bulletin. 111 (2), 275-290 (1992).
  68. Papini, M. R., Bitterman, M. E. The two-test strategy in the study of inhibitory conditioning. Psychological Review. 97 (3), 396-403 (1993).
  69. Sosa, R., Ramírez, M. N. Conditioned inhibition: Critiques and controversies in the light of recent advances. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior and Cognition. , (2018).
  70. Fox, A. T., Hand, D. J., Reilly, M. P. Impulsive choice in a rodent model of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Behavioural Brain Research. 187, 146-152 (2008).
  71. Foscue, E. P., Wood, K. N., Schramm-Sapyta, N. L. Characterization of a semi-rapid method for assessing delay discounting in rodents. Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 101, 187-192 (2012).
  72. Brucks, D., Marshall-Pescini, S., Wallis, L. J., Huber, L., Range, F. Measures of Dogs’ Inhibitory Control Abilities Do Not Correlate across Tasks. Frontiers in Psychology. 8, (2017).
  73. McDonald, J., Schleifer, L., Richards, J. B., de Wit, H. Effects of THC on Behavioral Measures of Impulsivity in Humans. Neuropsychopharmacology. 28 (7), 1356-1365 (2003).
  74. Reynolds, B., Ortengren, A., Richards, J. B., de Wit, H. Dimensions of impulsive behavior: Personality and behavioral measures. Personality and Individual Differences. 40 (2), 305-315 (2006).
  75. Dellu-Hagedorn, F. Relationship between impulsivity, hyperactivity and working memory: a differential analysis in the rat. Behavioral and Brain Functions. 2 (10), 18 (2006).
  76. López, P., Alba, R., Orduña, V. Individual differences in incentive salience attribution are not related to suboptimal choice in rats. Behavior and Brain Research. 341 (2), 71-78 (2017).
  77. Ho, M. Y., Al-Zahrani, S. S. A., Al-Ruwaitea, A. S. A., Bradshaw, C. M., Szabadi, E. 5-Hydroxytryptamine and impulse control: prospects for a behavioural analysis. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 12 (1), 68-78 (1998).
  78. Sagvolden, T., Russell, V. A., Aase, H., Johansen, E. B., Farshbaf, M. Rodent models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry. 57, 9 (2005).
  79. Tomie, A., Aguado, A. S., Pohorecky, L. A., Benjamin, D. Ethanol induces impulsive-like responding in a delay-of-reward operant choice procedure: impulsivity predicts autoshaping. Psychopharmacology. 139 (4), 376-382 (1998).
  80. Monterosso, J., Ainslie, G. Beyond discounting: possible experimental models of impulse control. Psychopharmacology. 146 (4), 339-347 (1999).
  81. Burguess, M. A., Rabbit, P. . Methodology of frontal and executive function. , 81-116 (1997).
  82. Watterson, E., Mazur, G. J., Sanabria, F. Validation of a method to assess ADHD-related impulsivity in animal models. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 252, 36-47 (2015).
  83. Hackenberg, T. D. Of pigeons and people: some observations on species differences in choice and self-control. Brazilian Journal of Behavior Analysis. 1 (2), 135-147 (2005).
  84. Asinof, S., Paine, T. A. The 5-choice serial reaction time task: A task of attention and impulse control for rodents. Journal of Visualized Experiments. (90), e51574 (2014).
  85. Masaki, D., et al. Relationship between limbic and cortical 5-HT neurotransmission and acquisition and reversal learning in a go/no-go task in rats. Psychopharmacology. 189, 249-258 (2006).
  86. Bari, A., et al. Prefrontal and monoaminergic contributions to stop-signal task performance in rats. The Journal of Neuroscience. 31, 9254-9263 (2011).
  87. Flagel, S. B., Watson, S. J., Robinson, T. E., Akil, H. Individual differences in the propensity to approach signals vs goals promote different adaptations in the dopamine system of rats. Psychopharmacology. 191, 599-607 (2007).
  88. Swann, A. C., Lijffijt, M., Lane, S. D., Steinberg, J. L., Moeller, F. G. Trait impulsivity and response inhibition in antisocial personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 43 (12), 1057-1063 (2009).
  89. Lawrence, A. J., Luty, J., Bogdan, N. A., Sahakian, B. J., Clark, L. Impulsivity and response inhibition in alcohol dependence and problem gambling. Psychopharmacology. 207 (1), 163-172 (2009).
  90. Dougherty, D. M., et al. Behavioral impulsivity paradigms: a comparison in hospitalized adolescents with disruptive behavior disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 44 (8), 1145-1157 (2003).
  91. Rosval, L., et al. Impulsivity in women with eating disorders: Problem of response inhibition, planning, or attention. International Journal of Eating Disorders. 39 (7), 590-593 (2006).
  92. Huddy, V. C., et al. Reflection impulsivity and response inhibition in first-episode psychosis: relationship to cannabis use. Psychological Medicine. 43 (10), 2097-2107 (2013).

Play Video

Citar este artigo
Sosa, R., Saavedra, P., Niño de Rivera, R., Lago, G., Moreno, P., Galicia-Castillo, O., Hernández-Guerrero, C., Buenrostro-Jáuregui, M. Three Laboratory Procedures for Assessing Different Manifestations of Impulsivity in Rats. J. Vis. Exp. (145), e59070, doi:10.3791/59070 (2019).

View Video