This paper describes a novel protocol that combines the pharmacological manipulation of memory and radio telemetry to document and quantify the role of cognition in navigation.
An animal's ability to perceive and learn about its environment plays a key role in many behavioral processes, including navigation, migration, dispersal and foraging. However, the understanding of the role of cognition in the development of navigation strategies and the mechanisms underlying these strategies is limited by the methodological difficulties involved in monitoring, manipulating the cognition of, and tracking wild animals. This study describes a protocol for addressing the role of cognition in navigation that combines pharmacological manipulation of behavior with high-precision radio telemetry. The approach uses scopolamine, a muscarinic acetylcholine receptor antagonist, to manipulate cognitive spatial abilities. Treated animals are then monitored with high frequency and high spatial resolution via remote triangulation. This protocol was applied within a population of Eastern painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) that has inhabited seasonally ephemeral water sources for ~100 years, moving between far-off sources using precise (± 3.5 m), complex (i.e., non-linear with high tortuosity that traverse multiple habitats), and predictable routes learned before 4 years of age. This study showed that the processes used by these turtles are consistent with spatial memory formation and recall. Together, these results are consistent with a role of spatial cognition in complex navigation and highlight the integration of ecological and pharmacological techniques in the study of cognition and navigation.
Cognition (herein defined as "all processes involved in acquiring, storing, and using information from the environment" 1) is central to an array of complex navigation tasks2. For example, Sandhill cranes (Grus canadensis) show a marked improvement in migratory precision with experience3, and sea turtle species imprint on their natal beaches as hatchlings and return as adults4-6. Similarly, successful migration, dispersal, and foraging hinge on an animal's ability to gather information about their spatial environment7,8. Some animals appear to learn navigational routes in relation to specific landscape features9 and may use spatial cognition when moving between nesting and foraging areas10. Recent work on Eastern Painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) suggests a critical period in navigation, where successful navigation of upland habitat as adults hinges on juvenile experience within a narrow age range (< 4 years old11-13). Though together these studies demonstrate the progress that has been made in understanding the role of learning in navigation4-6, 14-16, the mechanisms that underlie such behaviors and the full role of cognition in navigation remain enigmatic, especially in vertebrates8, 17, 18.
Field investigations into the role of cognition in navigation are rare2, 8, 18, due largely to the methodological difficulties involved in monitoring, manipulating, and tracking wild animals. For example, the large spatial and temporal scales on which many animals navigate often preclude investigating both type of information that those animals potentially learn and how that information is acquired. Experimenters often face the logistical difficulties of detecting and locating animals when monitoring behavior over such large areas and time frames, thereby limiting the type of data that can be collected and the conclusions that can be drawn. Although the use of animal-mounted global positioning system (GPS) recorders may improve the probability of detection of widely ranging animals, spatial data collected by these means are generally of very coarse resolution and lack a detailed behavioral component. Consequently, the data that can be collected under such circumstances are of limited value for examining subtle variation in behavior among different groups or experimental treatments. Similarly, the direct, controlled manipulation of target behaviors is often prohibited by the spatial and temporal scales typical of navigation behaviors, as well as by inherent logistical constraints of field studies. Finding animals in their natural habitat, catching and manipulating them, and then collecting behavioral data without inadvertently producing spurious behaviors are major challenges of working with animals in the field. Therefore, the design of experiments on free-ranging animals is often constrained and the ability to conduct rigorous, controlled field experiments on the role of cognition in navigation is limited.
The present study circumvents many of the previous difficulties of investigating the relationship between cognition and navigation in the field by using a novel combination of pharmacological manipulation and high-resolution tracking of freely navigating animals under field conditions. Scopolamine, a muscarinic acetylcholine receptor (mAChR) antagonist, has been shown to block spatial memory formation and recall by blocking cholinergic activity in the brains of a variety of vertebrate taxa18-24. Scopolamine can be used effectively on free-ranging animals under field conditions11, 18 and has a marked but temporary effect (e.g., 6 – 8 hr in reptiles). Methylscopolamine, a mAChR antagonist that does not cross the blood-brain-barrier19-21, can be used to control for the possible peripheral effects of scopolamine and for non-cognitive aspects of behavior11. Pharmacology allows for the precise manipulation of cognition by directly affecting receptors, and high-precision radio telemetry allows for the observation of the resulting effects on behavior. Measurements taken via remote triangulation with both high spatial (± 2.5 m) and temporal (15 min) resolution allow for the precise documentation and quantification of animal behavior relative to the experimental manipulation of cognition.
This study11 was conducted between May and August 2013 and 2014 at Chesapeake Farms, a 3,300 acre wildlife management and agriculture research area in Kent Co., MD, USA (39.194°N, 76.187°W). The protocol involves five main steps: (1) capturing and handling animals (2) affixing radio transmitters (3) preparing the pharmacological agents (4) monitoring and manipulating animal movements, and (5) analyzing spatial data. The study described herein focused on the Eastern painted turtle (Chrysemys picta). Turtles in the focal population engage in annual overland movements in which they leave their home ponds and navigate to alternative aquatic habitats using one of four very precise (± 3.5 m), complex, and highly predictable routes11, 12. Pharmacological manipulation of animals in this system paired with high-resolution radio telemetry sheds light on the role of cognition in freely navigating wild animals.
All procedures involving animal subjects were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committees of Franklin and Marshall and Washington Colleges and followed all local, state, and federal regulations.
1. Capture and Handling
2. Affixing Radio Transmitter
3. Pharmacological Preparation
Caution: Scopolamine hydrobromide and scopolamine methylbromide are potent acetylcholine antagonists. When working with these drugs, consult the Materials Safety Data Sheet, use proper personal protective equipment (e.g., gloves, fume hood), and follow laboratory safety protocols to avoid accidental contact.
4. Track Turtle Movements Using Radio Telemetry11, 12
5. Spatial Analysis
Using the above protocol, the role of cognition in navigation was assessed in a population of Eastern painted turtles (Chrysemys picta) that has experienced seasonal ephemeral water sources for ~100 years. This population inhabits a mix of ephemeral (drained annually and rapidly – in several hr) and permanent aquatic habitats (Figure 1). Previous studies suggest that after their ponds are drained, resident turtles navigate to alternative water sources with high precision (± 3.5 m) using complex, predictable routes learned before 4 years of age11-13 (Figure 1).
This study showed that the processes used by these turtles are consistent with spatial memory formation and recall11. Scopolamine blocked cholinergic activity in the brains of animals (including spatial memory formation and recall19-21) during navigation. Experienced adults treated with scopolamine diverted from their precise historic routes while naïve juveniles lacking experience (and thus memory) were unaffected by scopolamine, suggesting no effect of scopolamine on perceptual or non-spatial cognitive processes11 (Figures 1 and 2). Further, neither adult nor juvenile navigation was affected by the methylscopolamine control. Adult animals (i.e., those with previous experience at the site) injected with scopolamine lost their ability to follow the historic paths and the juveniles that use local cues to navigate and those adults injected with the drug that does not cross the blood-brain barrier were unaffected. Therefore, navigation in adults in this system seems to be cognitive in nature. Together, these results are consistent with the idea that turtles have a critical period during which they must learn paths and use cholinergic-dependent cognitive systems (spatial memory) to navigate as adults11-13.
Figure 1. Navigation is Based on Cognitive Processing in Adult Turtles. Representative movements of (a) experienced adults and (b) naïve juveniles (1 – 3 years) from temporary (T) to permanent (P) ponds while treated with either scopolamine or methylscopolamine. All adults receiving scopolamine (a, yellow, n = 9) drifted dramatically away (over 200 m) from the traditional routes (red, p< 0.001), while all naïve juveniles treated with the drug (b, yellow, n = 7) maintained movement exactly within traditional routes (p > 0.999). All control adults (a, white, n = 9) and control naïve juveniles (b, white, n = 6) followed traditional routes (p > 0.999). Each line of points represents one individual. All turtles from all groups maintained high precision prior to injection (p > 0.999). Data from Roth and Krochmal11. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 2. The Precision of Navigation is a Function of Cognitive Processing in Adult Turtles. a) All turtles demonstrated high precision of movement prior to injection of the treatment (scopolamine) or control (methylscopolamine; p > 0.999). b) After injection, adults in the scopolamine treatment deviated significantly (p < 0.001) from their traditional routes. In contrast, all other groups continued to navigate with high precision (± 3.5 m; p > 0.999). Inserts show detail of overlap from 0.5 – 3.5 m. Points are means ± SEM. Data from Roth and Krochmal11. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
The protocol presented here allows the experimenter to document and quantify the role of cognition in navigation. Manipulating cognition in the field has proven difficult, as most approaches leave experimenters unable to know which specific aspects of the animal's behavior are being manipulated. However, the protocol presented here allows the experimenter to accurately manipulate and thus assess the role of cognition in navigation. The technique further allows experimenters to monitor animal navigation in real-time with exceptionally high spatial and temporal resolution, thereby empowering researchers to clearly document the behavioral ramifications of experimental manipulation of cognition in wild animals.
Within this context, radio telemetry provides the ability to precisely monitor animal movements over great distances, yielding both high-quality spatial and behavioral data. Although this application of telemetry is by no means new28, the majority of studies use this technique to address coarse questions in ecology and behavior (e.g., habitat use, home range size, etc.). The frequent monitoring of the animal location (4 – 5 times per hr) described here combined with fine scale spatial analyses provide a more detailed behavioral component to an animal's location in space. Note that the optimal tracking distance will be a function of the transmitter strength and the equipment sensitivity. Generally it is best practice to remain at least 25 m away from the animal to avoid disturbing it, although when the animal is located in open vegetation, the distance needed to avoid such disturbance might be larger.
In the current application, high-precision radio telemetry offers unique advantages over the use of animal-mounted GPS recorders. Transmitters can be smaller, are less expensive, and have longer battery life than GPS units28. Moreover, the temporal resolution of remote triangulation via radio telemetry is far superior to animal-mounted GPS. Temporally, animal-mounted GPS units are limited by battery life (i.e., a finite number of measurements can be taken, thereby constraining their frequency). High-precision tracking with GPS would require a large battery to obtain high frequency position over a long period of time. The substantial mass of these batteries preclude them from use in small animal-mounted GPS units28. Furthermore, high-precision radio telemetry is not constrained by expensive data retrieval costs, or limited by on-board memory storage. However, radio telemetry is not optimal for tracking animals with particularly large movement ranges (e.g., during long-distance migration), in deep water or fossorial species, or those in steep montane habitats. In addition, high-precision radio tracking can be very time intensive and requires a relatively large field crew, particularly for fast-moving species; therefore, this approach may not suitable for all questions.
Pharmacological manipulation with scopolamine and methylscopolamine offers specific advances for the study of cognition in a natural setting. Behavior can be hard to interpret, especially under field conditions, thereby limiting the scope of potential inquiry. Scopolamine allows the manipulation of specific receptors that influence cognitive processes, enabling the researcher to ask questions specifically about the manipulation of cognition. Furthermore, as scopolamine easily crosses the blood-brain barrier and methylscopolamine does not, researchers can control for peripheral effects of scopolamine thereby dissociating cognitive-based from non-cognitive behaviors. These benefits of pharmacological manipulation allow for the generation and subsequent testing of clear behavioral predictions and afford the use of complex experimental designs under field conditions. However, scopolamine is a very general acetylcholine antagonist that can have unintended effects on other behavioral, sensory, and cognitive systems21-24. Therefore, it is possible that the use of scopolamine can produce effects that can interfere with the interpretation of complex behaviors (e.g., pupil dilation, thermal sensitivity21-24, 29, 30); no such confounding effects have been detected in this or previous studies11-13, 19, 20.
Common problems encountered while radio tracking include weak signal, loss of signal, and interference. To combat a weak signal, increase gain, change antenna orientation, move closer to the animal (being careful to avoid disturbing the anima), and elevate the antenna28. If the signal is lost completely, search with the gain and antenna as high as possible in an outwardly spiraling area restricted search28. Interference can be combated by decreasing gain, using the attenuator or noise cancelling filter (if so equipped), and changing antenna orientation. If interference cannot be overcome by these means, future work at the study site should focus on bandwidths that are not affected by interference.
Overall, pharmacological manipulation in conjunction with high-precision telemetry provides unique insight into the role that cognition plays in the origin and manifestation of navigation. The novelty of this unique method allows researchers to better understand the underlying neurological mechanisms that give rise to cognition in navigation. Moreover, these techniques can be used for additional studies of cognition in the wild with particular applicability to spatially explicit behaviors (e.g., navigation, migration, foraging, and dispersal)11-13, 33, the evolution of cognition1, 7, and conservation (e.g., translocation, reintroduction)31, 32. This technique is useful for a wide range of taxa in a wide range of habitats and will be vital to understanding phylogenic patterns in cognition.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
This research was funded by Washington College’s Provost’s Office, Middendorf Fund, Hodson Trust, and Franklin and Marshall’s Hackman Fund and College of Grants. We thank E. Counihan, S. Giordano, F. Rauh, and A. Roth for assistance in the field. We thank M. Conner, R. Fleegle, and D. Startt at Chesapeake Farms, and Chino Farms for permission and access. The Washington College GIS Program helped with the preparation of maps.
Scopolamine bromide | Sigma | S0929 | USP |
Scopolamine methylbromide | Sigma | S8502, 1421009 | USP and non USP versions |
Saline | Hanna Pharmaceutical Supply Co., Inc. | 409488850 | USP, formulated as an injectable |
Syringe filter | Fisher | 09-720-004 | |
Syringe | Fisher | 14-823-30 | |
Hypodermic needle | Fisher | 14-823-13 | |
Antenna | Wildlife Materials | 3 Element Folding Yagi | Antennae with additional elements are available, but can be cumbersome in the field. |
Radio Receiver | Wildlife Materials | TRX-2000S | Water resistant models are also available. |
Compass | Brunton | Truarc 15 | |
Radio transmitters | Holohil Inc. | BD-2, PD-2, RI-2B | Transmitter models vary in lifespan and signal output as a function of battery size and pulse rate settings, which can be customized based on the study question and organism. |
GPS | Garmin | eTrex Venture | |
Coaxial cable | newegg.com | C2G 40026 | BNC connections are necessary. |
Hoop net | Memphis Net and Twine | TN325 | Net mesh size should be chosen based on the minimum size of the target animal. |