Summary

Pupillometry to Assess Auditory Sensation in Guinea Pigs

Published: January 06, 2023
doi:

Summary

Pupillometry, a simple and non-invasive technique, is proposed as a method to determine hearing-in-noise thresholds in normal hearing animals and animal models of various auditory pathologies.

Abstract

Noise exposure is a leading cause of sensorineural hearing loss. Animal models of noise-induced hearing loss have generated mechanistic insight into the underlying anatomical and physiological pathologies of hearing loss. However, relating behavioral deficits observed in humans with hearing loss to behavioral deficits in animal models remains challenging. Here, pupillometry is proposed as a method that will enable the direct comparison of animal and human behavioral data. The method is based on a modified oddball paradigm – habituating the subject to the repeated presentation of a stimulus and intermittently presenting a deviant stimulus that varies in some parametric fashion from the repeated stimulus. The fundamental premise is that if the change between the repeated and deviant stimulus is detected by the subject, it will trigger a pupil dilation response that is larger than that elicited by the repeated stimulus. This approach is demonstrated using a vocalization categorization task in guinea pigs, an animal model widely used in auditory research, including in hearing loss studies. By presenting vocalizations from one vocalization category as standard stimuli and a second category as oddball stimuli embedded in noise at various signal-to-noise ratios, it is demonstrated that the magnitude of pupil dilation in response to the oddball category varies monotonically with the signal-to-noise ratio. Growth curve analyses can then be used to characterize the time course and statistical significance of these pupil dilation responses. In this protocol, detailed procedures for acclimating guinea pigs to the setup, conducting pupillometry, and evaluating/analyzing data are described. Although this technique is demonstrated in normal-hearing guinea pigs in this protocol, the method may be used to assess the sensory effects of various forms of hearing loss within each subject. These effects may then be correlated with concurrent electrophysiological measures and post-hoc anatomical observations.

Introduction

Pupil diameter (PD) can be affected by a wide number of factors and the measurement of PD that changes over time is known as pupillometry. PD is controlled by the iris sphincter muscle (involved in constriction) and the iris dilator muscle (involved in dilation). The constriction muscle is innervated by the parasympathetic system and involves cholinergic projections, whereas the iris dilator is innervated by the sympathetic system involving noradrenergic and cholinergic projections1,2,3. The best-known stimulus to induce PD changes is luminance-constriction and dilation responses of the pupil can be produced by variations in ambient light intensity2. PD also changes as a function of focal distance2. It has been known for decades, however, that PD also shows non-luminance-related fluctuations4,5,6,7. For example, changes in internal mental states can elicit transient PD changes. The pupil dilates in response to emotionally charged stimuli or increases with arousal4,5,8,9. Pupil dilation could also be related to other cognitive mechanisms, such as increased mental effort or attention10,11,12,13. Because of this relationship between pupil size variations and mental states, PD changes have been explored as a marker of clinical disorders such as schizophrenia14,15, anxiety16,17,18, Parkinson's disease19,20, and Alzheimer's disease21, among others. In animals, PD changes track internal behavioral states and are correlated with neuronal activity levels in cortical areas22,23,24,25. Pupil diameter has also been shown to be a reliable indicator of the sleep state in mice26. These PD changes related to arousal and the internal state typically occur on long time scales of the order of several tens of seconds.

In the domain of hearing research, in normal hearing as well as in hearing impaired subjects, listening effort and auditory perception have been assessed using pupillometry. These studies typically involve trained research subjects27,28,29,30 that perform various kinds of detection or recognition tasks. Because of the aforementioned relationship between arousal and PD, increased task engagement and listening effort have been shown to be correlated with increased pupil dilation responses30,31,32,33,34,35. Thus, pupillometry has been used to demonstrate that increased listening effort is expended to recognize spectrally degraded speech in normal-hearing listeners29,36. In hearing impaired listeners, such as humans with age-related hearing loss27,30,37,38,39,40,41 and cochlear implant users42,43, pupil responses also increased with decreasing speech intelligibility; however, hearing impaired listeners showed greater pupil dilation in easier listening conditions compared to normal hearing subjects27,30,37,38,39,40,41,42,43. But experiments that require the listener to perform a recognition task are not always possible – for example, in infants, or in some animal models. Thus, non-luminance related pupil responses evoked by acoustic stimuli could be a viable alternative method to assess auditory detection in these cases44,45. Earlier studies demonstrated a transient and stimulus-linked pupil dilation as part of the orienting reflex46. Later studies have demonstrated the use of stimulus-linked pupil dilations to derive frequency sensitivity curves in owls47,48. Recently, these methods have been adapted to assess sensitivity of the pupil dilation response in human infants48. Pupillometry has been shown to be a reliable and non-invasive approach to estimate auditory detection and discrimination thresholds in passively listening guinea pigs (GPs) by using a wide range of simple (tones) and complex (GP vocalizations) stimuli49. These stimulus-related PD changes typically occur at faster time scales of the order of several seconds and are linked to stimulus timing. Here, pupillometry of stimulus-related PD changes is proposed as a method to study behavioral impacts of various kinds of hearing impairment in animal models. In particular, pupillometry protocols for use in GPs, a well-established animal model of various types of auditory pathologies50,51,52,53,54,55,56 (also see reference57 for an exhaustive review) is described.

Although this technique is demonstrated in normal-hearing GPs, these methods can be easily adapted to other animal models and animal models of various auditory pathologies. Importantly, pupillometry can be combined with other non-invasive measurements such as EEG, as well as with invasive electrophysiological recordings in order to study the mechanisms underlying possible sound detection and perception deficits. Finally, this approach can also be used to establish broad similarities between human and animal models.

Protocol

For all experimental procedures, obtain approval from the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) and adhere to NIH Guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals. In the United States of America, GPs are additionally subject to United States Department of Agriculture (USDA) regulations. All the procedures in this protocol were approved by the University of Pittsburgh IACUC and adhered to NIH Guidelines for the care and use of laboratory animals. For this experiment, three male wildtype, pigmented GPs between 4 and 10 months of age, with ~600-1,000 g weight were used.

1. Surgical procedure

  1. Perform all pupillometry experiments in awake, head-fixed, and passively listening pigmented GPs. Verify normal hearing in experimental subjects using click and pure-tone auditory brainstem response (ABR) recordings58.
    NOTE: Although pupillometry data acquisition is by itself non-invasive, an invasive head post implant surgery is used in this protocol for immobilizing the animal's head during the procedure. Alternatives are presented in the Discussion section.
  2. First, implant all the experimental animals with a stainless-steel head post for head fixation under isoflurane anesthesia. Use aseptic surgical techniques to anchor the head post to the skull employing a combination of bone screws and dental acrylic58.
  3. Provide the animals with post-surgical care, including the administration of systemic and topical analgesics. After a 2-week recovery period, gradually acclimatize animals to the experimental setup.
    NOTE: The surgical procedure is based on previously published methods in GPs58 as well as other species59,60, and is not the focus of this protocol.

2. Animal acclimation to the experimental setup

NOTE: Experiments typically take place in a sound-attenuated chamber or booth (see Table of Materials). The time required to familiarize an animal to the setup varies from subject to subject. Typical acclimation times are noted below. A well-acclimated animal will tolerate head-fixation with minimal body motion, and result in better pupil diameter measurements.

  1. After a 2-week recovery period, first familiarize the animals to handling and transport (2-3 days). This acclimation is essential to reduce stress and anxiety. To familiarize the animal to handling, place the animal in its transport container for increasing amounts of time (10-30 min), and handle the animal for increasing amounts of time (10-30 min).
  2. Next, acclimate the animal to the experimental setup (2-3 days) by placing the animal in an enclosure for 10-45 min (Figure 1A). The enclosure must allow for small postural shifts for the animal's comfort during the experiment. Allow for small postural shifts for the animal's comfort during the experiment. However, pupil dilation is known to precede motion49. Therefore, measure the motion of the animal and account for this motion in the data analysis (Figure 1C).
  3. As part of this acclimation, manually handle the implanted head post, as if the animal is going to be head fixed. Hold the head post for increasing durations (10-60 s).
  4. After manual acclimation and depending on animal behavior, try to head fix the animal to a rigid frame using the implant holder.
  5. Slowly increase the head fixation duration (10-45 min) until the animal is calm and relatively still while being head-fixed (2-3 days).
  6. Accustom the animal to the presence of the camera, IR light source, and white light source (1-2 days). Turn on the white light, gradually increasing the duration (10 min to 30 min).
  7. Accustom the animal to acoustic stimulation by playing a variety of sounds (e.g., pure tones, clicks, vocalizations) at different sound levels (1-2 days, concurrent with step 2.6). To minimize habituation to experimental stimuli, use sounds different from those planned for the pupillometry experiments in this step.

3. Calibration of pupil camera

NOTE: The camera used for pupillometry outputs a video via USB to the pupillometry software suite. From this video, the pupil diameter is extracted using an ellipse fit and user-adjustable threshold value by the pupillometry software suite (see Table of Materials). The software then interfaces with a digital-to-analog card. The card outputs an analog voltage value that is proportional to the pupil diameter. Calibration is needed to convert this voltage value back to pupil diameter in units of length.

  1. Place a sheet of paper containing images of black discs of known diameter at the same location where the GP's eye will be located during pupillometry. For GPs, PD is in the 4 mm range. Therefore, perform calibration using 3 mm, 4 mm, and 5 mm discs.
  2. Place the pupillometry camera (see Table of Materials) at the same distance (25 cm) at which the experiments will be performed. Adjust the camera aperture and focus until a sharply focused image of a disc of known diameter is obtained.
  3. In the pupillometry acquisition software (see Table of Materials), adjust the threshold so that the outline of the ellipse fit closely matches the imaged disc, and note down the analog output voltage value and scaling.
  4. Repeat this procedure for the 3 mm, 4 mm, and 5 mm discs. Then, tabulate the actual diameter values (in mm) corresponding to the analog output voltage values.

4. Pupillometry data acquisition

  1. Perform all the experiments in a sound-attenuated booth or chamber, with the inner walls covered with anechoic foam.
  2. For free-field stimulus delivery, mount a calibrated loudspeaker onto the sound attenuated chamber wall, at an equal height to the position where the animal will be placed.
    NOTE: The choice of loudspeaker depends on the species being studied and the stimuli planned. For GP vocalizations, use a full-range driver speaker that has a relatively flat (±3 dB) frequency response in the vocalization frequency range of 0.5-3 kHz (Figure 1A).
  3. Place the animal in the enclosure ensuring that large body movements are not possible (Figure 1A).Fix the head of the animal to the rigid frame as described in step 2 (Figure 1A).
  4. Place a piezoelectric sensor beneath the enclosure in order to detect and record animal movements (Figure 1A).
  5. To set up the air puff, use a holder attached to the tabletop to place a pipette tip at ~15 cm in front of the animal's snout. Connect a silicon tube (~3 mm diameter) to the pipette tip and connect the tube to a regulated air cylinder.
  6. Keep the cylinder air pressure between 20 and 25 psi. Pass the tube through a pinch valve to control the timing and duration of the air puff using a computer-controlled relay.
  7. Illuminate the eye with an infrared LED array placed at ~10 cm distance. Use white LED lighting at an intensity of ~2,000 cd/m2 to illuminate the imaged eye and bring the baseline PD to ~3.5 mm. Maintain constant illumination conditions in the experimental chamber across experimental sessions.
    NOTE: In normal laboratory lighting (~500 cd/m2), the GP pupil is quite dilated, and does not allow for the observation of further stimulus-linked dilation. By using additional illumination, the pupil is brought to a baseline diameter of ~3.5 mm, allowing for a sufficient dynamic range to observe stimulus-linked dilation. This also ensures consistent baselines across sessions and subjects.
  8. Open the pupil acquisition software and acquire the video (at 90 fps) of the pupil using a camera with a 16 mm lens (spatial resolution of 0.15° visual angle) and infrared (IR) filter placed at a 25 cm distance from the imaged eye. Ensure that the eye is centered in the imaged area.
  9. Regulate the aperture and focus of the camera, as well as the IR level until the outline of the imaged pupil is in sharp focus.
  10. In the pupil acquisition software, define the area of interest containing the pupil by selecting a rectangular area with the mouse.
  11. Use the pupil acquisition software controls panel to adjust the brightness and contrast of the acquired video. Set the scan density to 5 and adjust the threshold such that the ellipse fit closely matches the outline of the pupil in the video.
  12. Using the neural interface processor software, acquire and save the analog signal from the PD trace, the voltage trace from the piezoelectric sensor recording motion, the stimulus delivery times, and the air puff delivery times.

5. Call-in-noise detection and categorical discrimination using a modified oddball paradigm

NOTE: The stimuli for pupillometry experiments consisted of GP vocalizations that were recorded in an animal colony58. The vocalization samples can be found in the following repository: https://github.com/vatsunlab/CaviaVOX. In particular, wheek and whine calls were used to elicit the pupil responses shown in the representative results. From each category, choose vocalizations whose lengths are approximately equal. To account for differences in the recording amplitude and temporal envelopes of the vocalizations, normalize the vocalizations by their root mean square (r.m.s.) amplitudes, if needed.

  1. Present the auditory stimuli using MATLAB at an appropriate sampling rate. For GPs, which are low-frequency-hearing animals, a sampling rate of 100 kHz is sufficient.
  2. Select eight different exemplars of GP vocalizations of similar lengths from two different categories of vocalizations (e.g., wheek calls and whine calls). One category (eight exemplars) will serve as standard stimuli, and the other category (eight exemplars) will serve as the oddball or deviant stimuli (Figure 2A).
  3. To generate 1 s long standard and deviant stimuli embedded in noise at different signal-to-noise-ratio (SNR) levels, add white noise of equal length to the calls (gated noise). The range of SNRs sampled in this experiment is between -24 dB SNR and +40 dB SNR.
  4. Using a block design, in each experimental session (~12 min duration), acquire data corresponding to a single SNR level. In each session, use eight exemplars of one vocalization category at a particular SNR as standard stimuli, and eight exemplars of the other vocalization category at that same SNR level as deviant stimuli.
    NOTE: A typical experimental block lasts ~12 minutes. Depending on the animal's behavior and habituation of pupil responses, it may be possible to acquire data for 3-4 blocks each day (~45 – 60 mins). Throughout this duration, monitor the animal closely via the pupil video, the motion trace, as well as directly between blocks.
  5. For each session, prepare a pseudorandom stimulus presentation sequence that contains standard stimuli >90% of the time. Ensure that between deviant stimuli, there are at least 20 trials with standard stimuli (Figure 2B).
    NOTE: Depending on the experiment, the ordering of deviant stimuli within the stimulus presentation sequence can adopt a Latin square design to ensure that each unique deviant stimulus occupies a unique sequential position in every session. Averaging over all sessions can thus minimize the effect of the deviant stimulus position within the overall stimulus sequence.
  6. Use a fixed stimulus intensity (for example, 85 dB SPL) for all stimulus presentation.
    NOTE: Use an appropriate digital-to-analog converter to generate an audio signal, attenuate it to the desired sound level using a programmable attenuator, power-amplify the signal and deliver the signal using a calibrated loudspeaker (for example, hardware, see Table of Materials).
  7. Present the stimuli with high temporal regularity (1 s stimulus followed by 3 s of silence as shown in the representative results).
    NOTE: The pupil dilation responses are slow, typically peaking about 1 s after stimulus onset and taking about 5 s to return to baseline49. The stimulus presentation rate must be low enough to account for these slow timescales. Temporal regularity is important because it is possible that interrupting the timing pattern could itself act as a deviant stimulus.
  8. To maintain the animal's engagement with the stimuli and to minimize habituation, optionally deliver a brief air puff (100 ms) after the deviant stimulus. Ensure that the onset of the air puff is sufficiently separated from the stimulus duration (2.5 s from stimulus onset) so that stimulus-evoked pupil dilation responses reach a peak before air puff induced blink artifacts.
    ​NOTE: In the classical oddball paradigm, no positive or negative reinforcements are used. Since an air puff is used here as a mildly aversive reinforcement to maintain the animal's engagement with the auditory stimuli, the paradigm is referred to as a modified oddball paradigm.

6. Analysis and statistics

NOTE: All the analyses were performed using custom code written in MATLAB (available at https://github.com/vatsunlab/GP_Pupil). Two main analysis methods are described, which address the reliability and the time course of pupil responses, respectively. The choice of one or both the methods will be dictated by experimental design.

  1. Motion detection and trial exclusion
    1. Using the code pupil_avg_JOVE.m, perform motion detection and trial exclusion for every session. To do so, run the code and select the data file from a single session in the popup dialog.
    2. Linearly detrend the PD trace and convert the units from voltage to micrometers using the calibration table derived earlier (see step 3). Also, linearly detrend the motion trace over the entire recording session (~12 min).
    3. Inspect the session data by plotting the pupil trace (Figure 1B – top row) and the linearly detrended motion trace (Figure 1B– bottom row) over the session duration (~12 min), overlaid on trial markers.
    4. Measure the standard deviation (SD) of the motion trace. Obtain the times of motion trace peaks using the findpeaks function in MATLAB. Consider the peaks that crossed a threshold of 5 SDs and that are separate from other peaks by at least 1 s as a motion event49 (Figure 1B – bottom).
    5. Discard any trials (both standard and deviant) of pupil dilation that occurs within 7 s of a motion event. If more than half the number of deviant trials is discarded due to motion-related pupil dilation, discard the entire session and repeat it.
  2. Data pre-processing and visualization
    1. Use the code pupil_avg_JOVE.m, to remove eye blink artifacts, pre-process the data, and obtain the average pupil dilation to each stimulus across sessions. To do so, run the code and select all the data files to be analyzed in the popup dialog.
    2. Detect eye blinks (PD changes exceeding 400 µm/ms) and remove them by linearly interpolating the PD trace in a 200 ms time window centered at the detected blink time. Discard the session data if more than half the number of deviant trials contain an eye blink between stimulus onset and air puff onset.
    3. Downsample PD data from the acquisition sampling rate of 1,000 Hz to 10 Hz.
    4. Extract PD traces in a window beginning 1 s before the stimulus onset and lasting 5 s after stimulus offset. Compute the average baseline PD for each stimulus in a 500 ms window just prior to the onset of the stimulus. Subtract the baseline PD from these traces to obtain the stimulus-evoked change in PD.
    5. Average the stimulus-evoked PD changes for each stimulus condition across sessions within each animal, and then across animals to generate the mean pupil dilation response to each stimulus condition (for example, Figure 3A).
  3. Growth curve analysis (GCA) for quantifying the time course of PD changes
    NOTE: This analysis method determines the magnitude and time course of pupil dilation responses and has been used in pupillometric studies of human subjects27,36,40 as well as in guinea pigs49.
    1. Vertically concatenate all the outputs from pupil_avg_JOVE.m for all the sessions, animals, SNRs and attenuations to construct a matrix containing the following columns: animalID, SNR, sound level, and Pupil (1-50) diameter values. Using the code pupil_LME_JOVE.m, perform the growth curve analysis (GCA)27,36,40,49.
    2. Fit linear mixed-effect models with subject-level intercepts as random effects, and orthogonal time polynomials of up to order two as fixed effects, with each deviant SNR treated as a separate group, to the rising phase of the pupil diameter trace (0.1 to 2.1 s following stimulus onset).
    3. Model the rising phase of the pupil trace using the following formula36,49:
      Pupildilation = (Intercept + Condition) + time1 * (βtime1 + βtime1: Condition) + time2* (βtime2+ βtime2: Condition) + r(subjectlevelintercept)
      ​Where, time1 and time2 correspond to orthogonal linear and quadratic time polynomials, and βs correspond to weights.
    4. Estimate mean weights (βs) and their standard errors using the fitlme function in MATLAB. Estimate the statistical significance of the weights using the coeftest function.
    5. For each SNR, plot the weights corresponding to the intercept, linear, and quadratic terms to visualize the results (Figure 3B, C).
  4. Analysis of trials showing statistically significant pupil dilations
    NOTE: This analysis method determines the fraction of deviant trials on which a statistically significant pupil dilation response is observed and corresponds to the reliability of pupil dilation responses.
    1. Choose an appropriate analysis window (0.5-1 s) centered around the peak of the pupil response (usually ~1.5 s after stimulus onset). Compute the mean PD in this analysis window for all standard and deviant trials.
    2. Determine whether the mean PD for each of the deviant trials is greater than 2.33 standard errors of the pooled distribution of mean PD values for standard trials. Count the deviant trials that exceed this threshold as trials showing a significant pupil dilation.
    3. Divide the number of deviant trials showing a significant pupil dilation by the total number of deviant trials (for each condition) to quantify the fraction of trials that show statistically significant increases in PD compared to standard stimulus trials.
    4. Put all the session-wise percentage of trials with significant pupil changes into each cell of a cell-array, where the cells are arranged from lower to higher SNR. Using the code pupil_threshold_estimate_JOVE.m, estimate the call-in-noise-categorization threshold.
    5. Plot the fraction of trials that show a statistically significant increase in PD as a function of SNR (Figure 3D). To these data, use the fitnlm MATLAB function (in the statistics toolbox) to fit psychometric functions of the form61:
      Ψ(x; α, β, λ) = (1 -λ) * F(x; α, β)
      Where, F is the Weibull function, defined as
      F(x; α, β) = Equation 1, α is the shift parameter, β is the slope parameter, and λ is the lapse rate.

Representative Results

Pupillometry was performed in three male pigmented GPs, weighing ~600-1,000 g over the course of the experiments. As described in this protocol, to estimate call-in-noise categorization thresholds, an oddball paradigm was used for stimulus presentation. In the oddball paradigm, calls belonging to one category (whines) embedded in white noise at a given SNR were employed as standard stimuli (Figure 2A), and calls from another category (wheeks) embedded in white noise at the same SNR (Figure 2A) as deviant stimuli. Standard and deviant stimuli were randomly chosen, with resampling, from eight exemplars of each category. In each experimental session, stimuli were presented with high temporal regularity (Figure 2B), with at least 20 presentations of standard stimuli between deviant stimuli. Data were acquired corresponding to a particular SNR level in each experimental session. Across sessions, a broad range of clean and noisy SNRs were sampled (-24, -18, -12, -6, -3, 0, 3, 6, 12, 40 dB SNR).

The PD changes to the standard stimuli did not differ significantly from the baseline (blue line in Figure 3A). The deviant stimuli evoked robust and significantly larger PD changes than those elicited by the standard stimuli (gray lines in Figure 3A), reflecting call category discrimination. The response magnitude and the percentage of trials with statistically significant pupil responses were highest at the cleanest SNR and decreased gradually with decreasing SNR (Figure 3A,B). Using GCA, pupil responses to deviant stimuli were found to be statistically significant at SNRs above -18 dB (Figure 3C), which was taken to be the call-in-noise categorization threshold (green line in Figure 3A). The percentage of significant trials at each tested SNR level was well-fit by a psychometric function (Figure 3D). The SNR level necessary to reach the half-maximum of the psychometric curve was about -20 dB SNR (Figure 3D). Anecdotally, for this case, the reliability-based and time course-based metrics yielded similar values of call-in-noise categorization thresholds.

Figure 1
Figure 1: Pupillometry setup, and stimulus-evoked and motion-related PD changes. (A) The pupillometry setup with video frame images of sound evoked pupil dilation (top). The baseline PD is shown by dashed green circles. (B) An exemplar PD trace (top) and exemplar motion trace (bottom) from a single experimental session. Vertical black lines correspond to onset time deviant stimulus presentations. Red ticks correspond to automatically detected motion events. Gray horizontal dashed line corresponds to 5 SD threshold. (C) The PD changes (ΔPD) evoked by deviant stimulus (top) and related to motion events (bottom) from one experimental session. Stimulus onset is shown by vertical black line; the detection of motion event is shown by vertical red line. Note that pupil dilation onset precedes the onset of motion. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 2
Figure 2: Call spectrograms and call-in-noise categorization paradigm structure. (A) Representative spectrograms of a guinea pig whine and wheek, in clean conditions and at 0- and -18-dB SNR, respectively. Noisy calls were obtained by adding white noise. (B) Structure of the oddball paradigm used to estimate call-in-noise categorization thresholds. Whine calls were randomly chosen from eight exemplars and used as standard stimuli. Wheek calls were randomly chosen from eight exemplars and used as deviants. In each experimental session, the noise was added at a different SNR level (-24, -18, -12, -6, -3, 0, 3, 6, 12 dB SNR). The calls are 1 s long and the time between stimuli is 3 s. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Figure 3
Figure 3: Pupillometry estimates of call-in-noise detections and categorization thresholds. (A) Average pupil responses from three animals. Mean pupil responses to standard whine stimuli are represented by blue line, and shading corresponds to ±1 standard error of mean (s.e.m.). Gray lines and shading correspond to mean and ±1 s.e.m. of pupil responses evoked by deviant wheek stimuli. Gray shading intensity corresponds to SNR. Green line and shading correspond to average pupil trace at threshold SNR (about -18 dB SNR). Red vertical line corresponds to stimulus onset; orange vertical line corresponds to air puff onset; teal dashed lines correspond to GCA window (PD changes rising phase). (B) GCA fit to the rising phase of PD changes. Dots are mean pupil diameter in 100 ms time bins, whiskers correspond to ±1 s.e.m. Solid lines correspond to mixed-effects model fits. Line colors as in A. (C) GCA weight estimates. Weights of the intercept is in blue, slope is in red, and acceleration is in purple. Whiskers correspond to ±1 s.e.m. Asterisks show statistically significant regression weights (linear hypothesis test on linear regression model coefficients). (D) Psychometric function fit to the percent of trials with significant PD changes elicited by the deviant stimulus as a function of SNR. Whiskers correspond to ±1 s.e.m. Note that 50% of the maximum is reached at about -20 dB SNR (green dashed line). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Discussion

This protocol demonstrates the use of pupillometry as a non-invasive and reliable method to estimate auditory thresholds in passively listening animals. Following the protocol described here, call-in-noise categorization thresholds in normal hearing GPs were estimated. Thresholds estimated using pupillometry were found to be consistent with those obtained using operant training62. Compared to operant training, however, the pupillometry protocol was relatively straightforward and quick to set up and acquire data. Each data acquisition session (per SNR level) lasted about 12 min, which resulted in 1-2 h of experimental sessions (across SNR levels) per animal per day49. Data acquisition could be completed in about 7-10 days (depending on the number of SNR levels used). Although the oddball paradigm was used for call-in-noise categorization threshold estimation in this manuscript, this pupillometry protocol can be adapted to easier versions of oddball paradigms, where just one call exemplar is used, or to other stimulus paradigms using a wide range of complex or simple stimuli49.

The method is not without disadvantages. First, the current protocol requires the implant of a head post to fix the head during these experiments. Head post implant surgery and recovery would add a minimum of 2 weeks to the timeline of the experimental protocol. It is possible that this step can be avoided by using other methods of non-invasively immobilizing awake animals during experiments-for example, by using custom 3D-printed helmets63 or deformable thermoplastics64. Further experiments are necessary to explore these solutions. Second, animals could rapidly habituate to deviant stimuli as well, resulting in decreasing pupil dilation responses over the course of an experimental session. This effect could be minimized by restricting experimental sessions to short durations (~12 min) and presenting only a limited number (8) of deviant stimuli. Furthermore, an air puff delivered after the deviant stimuli can ensure that animals remain engaged with the auditory stimuli. Third, because of this rapid habituation, several days may be required to complete data acquisition. By only testing SNR values that densely sample the steepest parts of the psychometric curve, the total number of experimental days can be minimized. Fourth, animals may not keep still during experiments, or blink excessively or close their eyes during experiments. These factors are a function of species and acclimation and show a high degree of individual variability. GPs are naturally docile, and by acclimating them well to the experimental setup, motion and blink artifacts can be minimized. Spontaneous blinks and saccades are typically quite rare in guinea pigs49, but this could be a function of the species as well. Finally, as mentioned earlier, pupil dynamics in humans have been associated with a number of neuropsychiatric disorders. While the experimental animals used here are assumed to be neurotypical, this caveat must be kept in mind while interpreting results.

While one hardware implementation of pupillometry is described here (using a commercially available eye tracker and neural data acquisition system), the equipment required is expensive and not economical to scale-up. However, other custom solutions based on the same underlying principle of infrared-based eye tracking that are more cost-effective, are available. For example, one study used custom components and custom video processing algorithms to extract pupil diameter from the recorded video22,25. Recently developed deep-learning algorithms are also able to extract pupil diameter from videographic data65,66. These solutions could more than halve the cost of pupillometry rigs. The trade-off here is between expense and time-while commercial solutions are more expensive, they are turn-key solutions that can be used out of the box. On the other hand, custom solutions are cost-effective and scalable, but require expertise to set-up, and the time needed to develop custom analysis pipelines.

Although the protocol detailed here was performed in normal hearing GPs, pupillometry could be relatively easy to use in other animal models of hearing impairment with appropriate changes to stimulus type and parameters. This would allow for characterizing the effects of hearing loss across a range of stimulus types and species, which could potentially yield novel observations. Since pupillometry is a non-invasive technique that has also been used extensively in humans, by using the same stimuli used for animal subjects, pupillometry can be used to compare the effects of various auditory pathologies across species. For example, a recent meta-analysis in humans showed that speech-in-noise perception deficits arising from moderate noise exposure were best observed when complex and temporally varying stimuli were used67. The estimation of call-in-noise categorization thresholds by pupillometry demonstrated here could be used as one such task using complex stimuli to evaluate the effects of noise exposure in GPs. The assessment of hearing at a behavioral level using these methods would complement electrophysiological and anatomical methods and could be part of the standard toolkit for evaluating various known hearing disorders.

In conclusion, the following points are critical for successful acquisition of pupillometric data. First, to ensure high data yield, it is critical to familiarize the animals well to the experimental setup. A lack of patience in this step could degrade the quality of data that is eventually obtained or necessitate the repetition of several sessions to make up for the lost sessions. Second, to avoid luminance-related PD changes, it is important to perform experiments in constant illumination conditions, maintaining these conditions between sessions and subjects as much as possible. Third, to minimize the number of experimental sessions needed, it is important to perform pilot experiments to identify critical parameter ranges for dense sampling. Fourth, to minimize habituation of the animals to the stimuli, it is important to perform experiments in short sessions containing only a few presentations of deviant stimuli. An air puff may be additionally used to maintain high engagement with the auditory stimuli.

Declarações

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the NIH (R01DC017141), the Pennsylvania Lions Hearing Research Foundation, and funds from the Departments of Otolaryngology and Neurobiology, University of Pittsburgh.

Materials

Analog output board Measurement Computing Corporation, Norton, MA PCI-DDA02/12
Anechoic foam Sonex One, Pinta Acoustic, Minneapolis, MN
Condenser microphone Behringer, Willich, Germany C-2
Free-field microphone Bruel & Kjaer, Denmark) Type 4940 
Matlab Mathworks, Inc., Natick, MA 2018a version
Monocular remote camera and illuminator system Arrington Research, Scottsdale, AZ MCU902 Infrared LED array + camera with infrared filter
Multifunction I/O Device National Instruments, Austin, TX PCI-6229
Neural interface processor Ripple Neuro, Salt Lake City, UT SCOUT
Piezoelectric motion sensor SparkFun Electronics, Niwot, CO SEN-10293
Pinch valve Cole-Palmer Instrument Co., Vernon Hills, IL EW98302-02
Programmable attenuator Tucker-Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL PA5
Silicon Tubing Cole-Parmer ~3 mm
Sound attenuating chamber IAC Acoustics
Speaker full-range driver Tang Band Speaker, Taipei, Taiwan W4-1879
Stereo Amplifier Tucker-Davis Technologies, Alachua, FL SA1
Tabletop – CleanTop Optical TMC vibration control / Ametek, Peabody, MA
Viewpoint software ViewPoint, Arrington Research, Scottsdale, AZ

Referências

  1. Steinhauer, S. R., Siegle, G. J., Condray, R., Pless, M. Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 52 (1), 77-86 (2004).
  2. Strauch, C., Wang, C. A., Einhäuser, W., Vander Stigchel, S., Naber, M. Pupillometry as an integrated readout of distinct attentional networks. Trends in Neurosciences. 45 (8), 635-647 (2022).
  3. Turnbull, P. R., Irani, N., Lim, N., Phillips, J. R. Origins of Pupillary Hippus in the autonomic nervous system. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science. 58 (1), 197-203 (2017).
  4. Bradley, M. M., Miccoli, L., Escrig, M. A., Lang, P. J. The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology. 45 (4), 602-607 (2008).
  5. Oliva, M., Anikin, A. Pupil dilation reflects the time course of emotion recognition in human vocalizations. Scientific Reports. 8 (1), 4871 (2018).
  6. Privitera, C. M., Renninger, L. W., Carney, T., Klein, S., Aguilar, M. Pupil dilation during visual target detection. Journal of Vision. 10 (10), 3 (2010).
  7. Zekveld, A. A., Koelewijn, T., Kramer, S. E. The pupil dilation response to auditory stimuli: Current state of knowledge. Trends in Hearing. 22, 2331216518777174 (2018).
  8. Alamia, A., VanRullen, R., Pasqualotto, E., Mouraux, A., Zenon, A. Pupil-linked arousal responds to unconscious surprisal. The Journal of Neuroscience. 39 (27), 5369-5376 (2019).
  9. Wang, C. A., et al. Arousal effects on pupil size, heart rate, and skin conductance in an emotional face task. Frontiers in Neurology. 9, 1029 (2018).
  10. Hess, E. H., Polt, J. M. Pupil size in relation to mental activity during simple problem-solving. Science. 143 (3611), 1190-1192 (1964).
  11. Kahneman, D., Beatty, J. Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science. 154 (3756), 1583-1585 (1966).
  12. Lisi, M., Bonato, M., Zorzi, M. Pupil dilation reveals top-down attentional load during spatial monitoring. Biological Psychology. 112, 39-45 (2015).
  13. Zhao, S., Bury, G., Milne, A., Chait, M. Pupillometry as an objective measure of sustained attention in young and older listeners. Trends in Hearing. 23, 2331216519887815 (2019).
  14. Steinhauer, S. R., Hakerem, G. The pupillary response in cognitive psychophysiology and schizophrenia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 658, 182-204 (1992).
  15. Thakkar, K. N., et al. Reduced pupil dilation during action preparation in schizophrenia. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 128, 111-118 (2018).
  16. Bitsios, P., Szabadi, E., Bradshaw, C. M. Relationship of the ‘fear-inhibited light reflex’ to the level of state/trait anxiety in healthy subjects. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 43 (2), 177-184 (2002).
  17. Burkhouse, K. L., Siegle, G. J., Gibb, B. E. Pupillary reactivity to emotional stimuli in children of depressed and anxious mothers. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 55 (9), 1009-1016 (2014).
  18. Nagai, M., Wada, M., Sunaga, N. Trait anxiety affects the pupillary light reflex in college students. Neuroscience Letters. 328 (1), 68-70 (2002).
  19. Giza, E., Fotiou, D., Bostantjopoulou, S., Katsarou, Z., Karlovasitou, A. Pupil light reflex in Parkinson’s disease: evaluation with pupillometry. International Journal of Neuroscience. 121 (1), 37-43 (2011).
  20. You, S., Hong, J. H., Yoo, J. Analysis of pupillometer results according to disease stage in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Scientific Reports. 11 (1), 17880 (2021).
  21. Fountoulakis, K. N., St Kaprinis, G., Fotiou, F. Is there a role for pupillometry in the diagnostic approach of Alzheimer’s disease? a review of the data. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society. 52 (1), 166-168 (2004).
  22. McGinley, M. J., David, S. V., McCormick, D. A. Cortical membrane potential signature of optimal states for sensory signal detection. Neuron. 87 (1), 179-192 (2015).
  23. McGinley, M. J., et al. Waking state: Rapid variations modulate neural and behavioral responses. Neuron. 87 (6), 1143-1161 (2015).
  24. Schwartz, Z. P., Buran, B. N., David, S. V. Pupil-associated states modulate excitability but not stimulus selectivity in primary auditory cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology. 123 (1), 191-208 (2020).
  25. Vinck, M., Batista-Brito, R., Knoblich, U., Cardin, J. A. Arousal and locomotion make distinct contributions to cortical activity patterns and visual encoding. Neuron. 86 (3), 740-754 (2015).
  26. Yüzgeç, &. #. 2. 1. 4. ;., Prsa, M., Zimmermann, R., Huber, D. Pupil size coupling to cortical states protects the stability of deep sleep via parasympathetic modulation. Current Biology. 28 (3), 392-400 (2018).
  27. Kuchinsky, S. E., et al. Pupil size varies with word listening and response selection difficulty in older adults with hearing loss. Psychophysiology. 50 (1), 23-34 (2013).
  28. Winn, M. B., Wendt, D., Koelewijn, T., Kuchinsky, S. E. Best practices and advice for using pupillometry to measure listening effort: An introduction for those who want to get started. Trends in Hearing. 22, 2331216518800869 (2018).
  29. Zekveld, A. A., Kramer, S. E. Cognitive processing load across a wide range of listening conditions: insights from pupillometry. Psychophysiology. 51 (3), 277-284 (2014).
  30. Zekveld, A. A., Kramer, S. E., Festen, J. M. Cognitive load during speech perception in noise: the influence of age, hearing loss, and cognition on the pupil response. Ear and Hearing. 32 (4), 498-510 (2011).
  31. Koelewijn, T., Zekveld, A. A., Festen, J. M., Kramer, S. E. Pupil dilation uncovers extra listening effort in the presence of a single-talker masker. Ear and Hearing. 33 (2), 291-300 (2012).
  32. McCloy, D. R., Lau, B. K., Larson, E., Pratt, K. A. I., Lee, A. K. C. Pupillometry shows the effort of auditory attention switching. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 141 (4), 2440 (2017).
  33. Piquado, T., Isaacowitz, D., Wingfield, A. Pupillometry as a measure of cognitive effort in younger and older adults. Psychophysiology. 47 (3), 560-569 (2010).
  34. Reilly, J., Kelly, A., Kim, S. H., Jett, S., Zuckerman, B. The human task-evoked pupillary response function is linear: Implications for baseline response scaling in pupillometry. Behavior Research Methods. 51 (2), 865-878 (2019).
  35. Zekveld, A. A., Kramer, S. E., Festen, J. M. Pupil response as an indication of effortful listening: the influence of sentence intelligibility. Ear and Hearing. 31 (4), 480-490 (2010).
  36. Winn, M. B., Edwards, J. R., Litovsky, R. Y. The impact of auditory Spectral Resolution on Listening Effort Revealed by Pupil Dilation. Ear and Hearing. 36 (4), 153-165 (2015).
  37. Ayasse, N. D., Wingfield, A. A Tipping point in listening effort: Effects of linguistic complexity and age-related hearing loss on sentence comprehension. Trends in Hearing. 22, 2331216518790907 (2018).
  38. Koelewijn, T., Versfeld, N. J., Kramer, S. E. Effects of attention on the speech reception threshold and pupil response of people with impaired and normal hearing. Hearing Research. 354, 56-63 (2017).
  39. Kramer, S. E., Kapteyn, T. S., Festen, J. M., Kuik, D. J. Assessing aspects of auditory handicap by means of pupil dilatation. Audiology. 36 (3), 155-164 (1997).
  40. Kuchinsky, S. E., et al. Speech-perception training for older adults with hearing loss impacts word recognition and effort. Psychophysiology. 51 (10), 1046-1057 (2014).
  41. Wendt, D., Hietkamp, R. K., Lunner, T. Impact of noise and noise reduction on processing effort: A pupillometry study. Ear and Hearing. 38 (6), 690-700 (2017).
  42. Winn, M. B. Rapid release from listening effort resulting from semantic context, and effects of spectral degradation and cochlear implants. Trends in Hearing. 20, 2331216516669723 (2016).
  43. Winn, M. B., Moore, A. N. Pupillometry reveals that context benefit in speech perception can be disrupted by later-occurring sounds, especially in listeners with Cochlear implants. Trends in Hearing. 22, 2331216518808962 (2018).
  44. Selezneva, E., Brosch, M., Rathi, S., Vighneshvel, T., Wetzel, N. Comparison of pupil dilation responses to unexpected sounds in monkeys and humans. Frontiers in Psychology. 12, 754604 (2021).
  45. Wetzel, N., Buttelmann, D., Schieler, A., Widmann, A. Infant and adult pupil dilation in response to unexpected sounds. Developmental Psychobiology. 58 (3), 382-392 (2016).
  46. Sokolov, E. N. Higher nervous functions; the orienting reflex. Annual Review of Physiology. 25, 545-580 (1963).
  47. Bala, A. D., Takahashi, T. T. Pupillary dilation response as an indicator of auditory discrimination in the barn owl. Journal of Comparative Physiology A. 186 (5), 425-434 (2000).
  48. Bala, A. D. S., Whitchurch, E. A., Takahashi, T. T. Human auditory detection and discrimination measured with the pupil dilation Response. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology. 21 (1), 43-59 (2020).
  49. Montes-Lourido, P., Kar, M., Kumbam, I., Sadagopan, S. Pupillometry as a reliable metric of auditory detection and discrimination across diverse stimulus paradigms in animal models. Scientific Reports. 11 (1), 3108 (2021).
  50. Coomber, B., et al. Neural changes accompanying tinnitus following unilateral acoustic trauma in the guinea pig. European Journal of Neuroscience. 40 (2), 2427-2441 (2014).
  51. Fan, L., et al. Pre-exposure to lower-level noise mitigates cochlear synaptic loss induced by high-level noise. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 14, 25 (2020).
  52. Furman, A. C., Kujawa, S. G., Liberman, M. C. Noise-induced cochlear neuropathy is selective for fibers with low spontaneous rates. Journal of Neurophysiology. 110 (3), 577-586 (2013).
  53. Hickman, T. T., Hashimoto, K., Liberman, L. D., Liberman, M. C. Synaptic migration and reorganization after noise exposure suggests regeneration in a mature mammalian cochlea. Scientific Reports. 10 (1), 19945 (2020).
  54. Huetz, C., Guedin, M., Edeline, J. M. Neural correlates of moderate hearing loss: time course of response changes in the primary auditory cortex of awake guinea-pigs. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience. 8, 65 (2014).
  55. Lin, H. W., Furman, A. C., Kujawa, S. G., Liberman, M. C. Primary neural degeneration in the Guinea pig cochlea after reversible noise-induced threshold shift. Journal of the Association for Research in Otolaryngology. 12 (5), 605-616 (2011).
  56. Shi, L., et al. Ribbon synapse plasticity in the cochleae of Guinea pigs after noise-induced silent damage. PLoS One. 8 (12), 81566 (2013).
  57. Naert, G., Pasdelou, M. P., Le Prell, C. G. Use of the guinea pig in studies on the development and prevention of acquired sensorineural hearing loss, with an emphasis on noise. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America. 146 (5), 3743 (2019).
  58. Montes-Lourido, P., Kar, M., Pernia, M., Parida, S., Sadagopan, S. Updates to the guinea pig animal model for in-vivo auditory neuroscience in the low frequency regime. Hearing Research. 424, 108603 (2022).
  59. Gao, L., Wang, X. Intracellular neuronal recording in awake nonhuman primates. Nature Protocols. 15 (11), 3615-3631 (2020).
  60. Lu, T., Liang, L., Wang, X. Neural representations of temporally asymmetric stimuli in the auditory cortex of awake primates. Journal of Neurophysiology. 85 (6), 2364-2380 (2001).
  61. Wichmann, F. A., Hill, N. J. The psychometric function: I. Fitting, sampling, and goodness of fit. Perception & psychophysics. 63 (8), 1293-1313 (2001).
  62. Kar, M., et al. Vocalization categorization behavior explained by a feature-based auditory categorization model. bioRxiv. , 483596 (2022).
  63. Schaeffer, D. J., Liu, C., Silva, A. C., Everling, S. Magnetic resonance imaging of marmoset monkeys. ILAR Journal. 61 (2-3), 274-285 (2020).
  64. Drucker, C. B., Carlson, M. L., Toda, K., DeWind, N. K., Platt, M. L. Non-invasive primate head restraint using thermoplastic masks. Journal of Neuroscience Methods. 253, 90-100 (2015).
  65. Meyer, A. F., O’Keefe, J., Poort, J. Two distinct types of eye-head coupling in freely moving mice. Current Biology. 30 (11), 2116-2130 (2020).
  66. Nath, T., et al. Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviors. Nature Protocols. 14 (7), 2152-2176 (2019).
  67. DiNino, M., Holt, L. L., Shinn-Cunningham, B. G. Cutting through the noise: Noise-Induced cochlear synaptopathy and individual differences in speech understanding among listeners with normal audiograms. Ear and Hearing. 43 (1), 9-22 (2022).

Play Video

Citar este artigo
Pernia, M., Kar, M., Montes-Lourido, P., Sadagopan, S. Pupillometry to Assess Auditory Sensation in Guinea Pigs. J. Vis. Exp. (191), e64581, doi:10.3791/64581 (2023).

View Video