This manuscript describes a Morris water maze (MWM) protocol tailored for use with a commonly used mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease. The MWM is widely used in transgenic mouse models. Implementation of a procedure sensitive to the background strain of the mouse model is essential for detecting group differences.
The Morris water maze (MWM) is a commonly used task to assess hippocampal-dependent spatial learning and memory in transgenic mouse models of disease, including neurocognitive disorders such as Alzheimer’s disease. However, the background strain of the mouse model used can have a substantial effect on the observed behavioral phenotype, with some strains exhibiting superior learning ability relative to others. To ensure differences between transgene negative and transgene positive mice can be detected, identification of a training procedure sensitive to the background strain is essential. Failure to tailor the MWM protocol to the background strain of the mouse model may lead to under- or over- training, thereby masking group differences in probe trials. Here, a MWM protocol tailored for use with the F1 FVB/N x 129S6 background is described. This is a frequently used background strain to study the age-dependent effects of mutant P301L tau (rTg(TauP301L)4510 mice) on the memory deficits associated with Alzheimer’s disease. Also described is a strategy to re-optimize, as dictated by the particular testing environment utilized.
Transgenic mouse models have been instrumental in evaluating the pathophysiology of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), as well as the potential of therapeutic interventions. Cognitive tasks, such as the Morris water maze (MWM), are commonly used with these models to identify the molecular correlates of memory deficits and to assess the efficacy of pre-clinical drugs. It is crucial, however, that the dynamic range of the cognitive task be wide enough to detect subtle treatment effects. With mouse models of AD, cognitive deficits are typically age-dependent, and mice display progressive declines in performance (e.g., 1). Use of a sensitive cognitive task can allow detection of subtle differences earlier in the animal’s life, thereby reducing the costs associated with aging animals. For example, reducing the number of training trials in the hippocampal-dependent Barnes maze from 15 to 5 increased the difficulty of the task, resulting in the detection of deficits in the 3xTg model at an earlier age than previously reported 2. Earlier detection of deficits not only offers considerable time and cost savings, it also increases the likelihood that the molecular changes underlying cognitive deficits can be identified.
One factor influencing the sensitivity of cognitive tasks is the genetic background strain of the mouse model. For example, BALB/c mice exhibit superior performance in learning and memory tasks compared to other strains, such as the C57BL/6 3. The F1 FVB/N x 129S6 background is used for two of the most widely employed models of AD, the Tg2576 and rTg(TauP301L)4510 models. This strain exhibits superior learning ability in the MWM relative to other strains, including B6/SJL mice 4. Because of this superior learning ability, the use of a single probe after extensive training may mask group differences resulting from over-training. In addition, the sensitivity of probe trials may be age-dependent. We have previously shown that earlier probe trials, after limited hidden platform training, are more sensitive to differences in young Tg2576 compared to young transgene-negative littermate controls than are probe trials inserted after more extensive training 5. In contrast, probe trials following extensive training are more sensitive in older (20-25 month) Tg2576 mice compared to older littermates than are earlier probe trials 5. By interspersing probe trials throughout training, the likelihood that a sensitive trial will be identified is increased, particularly if longitudinal testing is performed and the sensitivity of a particular probe trial is age-dependent. Figure 1 shows the superior performance of F1 FVB/N x 129S6 mice under the protocol optimized for this strain as compared to mice of the B6/SJL background trained under a protocol with more extensive training.
The MWM is generally thought to provide reliable measures that are reproducible across both time and laboratories 6. For example, the primary protocol originally used by our Minnesota laboratory 1,7 was successfully implemented with minor modifications at West Virginia University 8. Similarly, equivalent levels of impairment were observed in rTg(TauP301L)4510 mice relative to control littermates if housed under pathogen-free or conventional conditions 9. However, the testing environment can influence the sensitivity of the MWM task. Factors such as room lighting, air vents, temperature gradients, and noises all contribute to environmental cues 4 that can ultimately influence performance. When our Minnesota laboratory and vivarium were moved to a new building, up to a 38% reduction in wild-type performance was observed, substantially reducing the dynamic range of the task and the ability to detect transgene-related deficits. This change in performance occurred despite designing the testing room to be of equivalent size and configuration, and using the same applied visual cues. A “re-optimizing” of the original protocol was required to increase the dynamic range of the MWM task in the new testing environment.
Here the original protocol tailored for use with the F1 FVB/N x 129S6 background 5 is described. Because some studies suggest stress is associated with poor MWM performance 10 and pre-handling can alleviate this stress-induced deficit in performance 11, a pre-handling protocol was designed to acclimate the mice to the introduction and removal of the pool prior to MWM testing. Following pre-handling, mice undergo visible platform training, in which a raised platform is marked with a flag. Visible platform training is used to identify mice with performance problems related to sensorimotor abnormalities. Using exclusion criteria described in the protocol section, performance-incompetent mice are removed from subsequent examinations of hidden platform training and probe trials. Impairments in hidden platform training and probe trials are interpreted as cognitive deficits because sensorimotor performance is factored out of the data. After completion of visible platform training, mice begin hidden platform training where the platform is submerged in water and remains in the same position relative to external cues. Trials in which the platform is removed (probe trials) are interspersed throughout hidden platform training to assess the influence of additional training. Because probe trials occur at the beginning of each day, before additional hidden platform training, probe trials measure the ability of the animal to remember the location of the platform following a 20 hr delay, considered a measure of reference memory 12. Finally, ways in which this original protocol was re-optimized when changes in the testing environment disrupted control performance are described.
All experimental procedures were conducted in accordance with the standards of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee (IACUC) and approved by West Virginia University’s IACUC.
1. Pre-handling
2. Visible Platform Training
3. Morris Water Maze Hidden Platform Training
4. Probe Trials
5. Analyses
6. Example of Re-optimization for a New Testing Environment
We have used the Morris water maze to study the effects of beta-amyloid (Tg2576 mice) and mutant P301L tau (rTg(TauP301L)4510 mice) on spatial reference memory (e.g., 1,5,7,8). Figure 3 is the representative result reported in our study examining the effect of adult-onset P301L tau expression on learning and memory 8, utilizing testing Environment A. To assess motor and visual capabilities, mice were compared across visible platform training blocks, where each training block consisted of 3 trials. Pathlength in visible platform training did not differ between controls and TauP301L mice (Figure 3A), suggesting transgene positive and negative mice exhibit comparable swimming and both groups can see the visual cue (flag) marking the platform. No mice were identified as performance-incompetent based on the exclusion criteria. Next, performance in hidden platform training block was compared, where each block consists of 1 day (4 trials) of training. As the mice learned the location of the platform, the pathlength and time to find the platform decreased. However, pathlength was significantly longer in TauP301L mice compared to controls at each training block (Figure 3B), suggesting spatial learning was impaired in TauP301L mice. Four probe trials, in which the platform was removed, were interspersed throughout hidden platform training and took place at the beginning of the day, prior to the start of hidden platform training. Thus, these probe trials measured spatial reference memory. Comparing across these four probe trials, Controls significantly improved with additional training (Figure 3C), as indicated by increased time in the target quadrant. In contrast, TauP301L mice did not improve with additional training. Thus, the greatest differences between the two groups at this age occurred at probe trial 4. These data indicate that P301L tau expression is associated with both spatial learning and spatial reference memory deficits. While the water maze task may be relatively stable to some procedural differences, the F1 FVB/N x 129S6 background strain may be particularly sensitive to certain environmental changes. The first protocol outlined was also successfully utilized in Environment B (e.g., 1,7). However, wild-type probe performance was significantly lower when the first protocol was used in a third location, Environment C. The re-optimized protocol significantly improved wild-type probe performance (Figure 4).
Figure 1: Optimization of Protocol for Background Strain. Wild-type 7-8 month old mice were trained using the same cues and testing environment. F1 FVB/N x 129S6 (N = 24) and B6/SJL (N = 16) mice first received 18 and 24 visible platform training trials respectively, delivered at 6 and 8 trials per day respectively. Both strains received 4 hidden platform training trials per day. For F1 FVB/N x 129S6 mice, probe trials were performed 20 hr following 8, 12, 16, and 24 training trials. For B6/SJL mice, probe trials were performed 20 hr following 12, 24, and 36 training trials.
Figure 2: Timeline. Mice received visible platform training for 3 days, 6 trials per day, followed by hidden platform training for 6 days, 4 trials per day. Four probe trials were performed 20 hr after 8, 12, 16, and 24 hidden training trials.
Figure 3: Representative Results for the Morris Water Maze. TauP301L mice carrying the human P301L tau gene were examined at approximately 6.5 months of age after three months of P301L tau expression (n = 41 tauP301L and n = 46 controls with an approximately equal number of males and females in each group). (A) Pathlength in visible platform training did not differ between controls and TauP301L mice (ps > 0.05). (B) During hidden platform training, TauP301L mice demonstrated significantly longer pathlengths across all training blocks (Transgene: F(1, 83) = 41.96, p <0.0001; Transgene × Block: F(5, 415) = 0.6141, p = 0.69). (C) Controls improved across the four probe trials, whereas TauP301L mice did not (Transgene: F(1, 83) = 29.1, p <0.0001; Transgene × Trial: F(3, 270) = 4.91, p = 0.008). Each training block consisted of 3 trials for visible platform training or 4 trials for hidden platform training. Tukey post-hoc analyses: *p <0.05; **p <0.01; ***p <0.001. Portions of Figure 3 reprinted from Hunsberger et al., Effect size of memory deficits in mice with adult-onset P301L tau expression, Behav Brain Res, Vol. 272, pp. 181-95. Copyright 2014, with permission from Elsevier.
Figure 4: Probe Scores Are Significantly Affected by Testing Environment and Training Protocol. Three groups of tau negative F1 FVB/N x 129S6 mice trained using various combinations of training protocol and testing environment generated significantly different probe scores following 8, 12, and 16 training trials (Group: F(2, 42) = 14.89, p <0.0001; Group x Trial: F(4, 84) = 1.10, p = 0.36). Mice trained utilizing Protocol 1 and the same set of applied cues displayed significantly lower probe scores under Environment C compared to Environment B (Environment: F(1, 25) = 28.58, p <0.0001; Environment x Trial: F(2, 50) = 1.93, p = 0.16). Mice trained under Environment C displayed significantly higher probe scores when modified cues and the re-optimized Protocol 2 were utilized compared to mice trained utilizing the original cues and Protocol 1 (Protocol: F(1, 30) = 15.32, p <0.001; Protocol x Trial: F(2, 60) = 0.91, p = 0.41). **p <0.001; ***p <0.0001.
Day 1 | Morris Water Maze – Cohort: | Date: _________ | ||||||
Platform in SE quadrant | Tester: Start time: Finish time: Water temp: | 1m max trial time/15s on platform/*placed on platform | ||||||
Group 1 | ||||||||
Trial 1 | ||||||||
Animal ID | Marking | Cage # | End time |
Release point |
Platform location |
Find platform? | Time | Notes (i.e. odd behavior) |
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
W | SE | |||||||
20 minute ITI | ||||||||
Trial 2 | ||||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
E | SE | |||||||
20 minute ITI | ||||||||
Trial 3 | ||||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
N | SE | |||||||
20 minute ITI | ||||||||
Trial 4 | ||||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE | |||||||
S | SE |
Table 1: Hidden Platform Run Sheet. An example run sheet in which the platform is located in the southeast (SE) quadrant is provided. The experimenter should write the mouse’s ID number, any identifying marks on the tail or ears, and the cage grouping. The release point is indicated on the sheet and should be pseudo-randomly determined as described in Step 3.3.2. Record whether the mouse finds the platform and the time to find the platform. Odd behavior, including floating or thigmotaxis, should noted.
The MWM task is widely used to assess spatial learning and memory. However, the robustness of this task can be influenced by many factors and requires optimization for both background strain and testing environment. As shown in Figure 4, the same training protocol and applied visual cues used in two different testing rooms (equivalent size and layout) yielded significantly different probe performance. Since many features of the testing room might contribute to spatial cues 4, it was speculated that the two rooms were significantly different in some unknown way which made the test more difficult. In modifying the extra-maze cues, we opted to effectively increase the number of visual cues. In addition, by minimizing the use of curtains, any auditory or olfactory cues that might exist may have been altered. The light level of the two testing rooms were equivalent, however the home cage light level in the new vivarium was lower, resulting in a higher light level differential when going from home cage to testing room. It has been suggested that BALB/c mice are able to perform the MWM task providing the light levels are sufficiently low 14. However, attempts to improve performance by lowering testing room light levels were unsuccessful (unpublished observations). To date, it is not known what factor(s) contributed to the decline in performance in the new testing environment, but the modified pre-handling, cues, and training protocol have resulted in a significant increase in probe scores.
If possible, it is advised to test a group of wild-type or control mice of the same background strain and age as the planned experiment to assess rate of learning and optimal probe placement for the particular testing environment. Ideally, the first probe is placed when approximately half of the mice are displaying a probable positive search bias (%-time in target≥35) and the last probe when most control mice show a positive search bias and have reached performance plateau. This strategy has been used to determine probe placement for superior performers (F1 FVB/N x 129S6) and a background which learned more slowly (mixed C57BL/6 x FVB/N x 129S6), where approximately half of the mice had target quadrant occupancies of ≥35 after 8 trials or 18 trials respectively with a group mean of approximately 35%-time for the first probe. Subsequent probes should be higher than the first if set in this fashion, ideally with at least a 15-point difference compared to baseline of 25%-time. If comparing probe scores which are moderately low (~35%-time), ensure that this represents a significant bias for the target and hence a valid difference, by comparing the target to non-target quadrants 4,5. In addition, the minimum group mean should not be substantially lower than 25%-time in the target quadrant, which would be considered baseline or chance performance. Comparing target to non-target occupancy may help to identify if there are room biases, e.g., mice spending more time in the opposite quadrant than either of the adjacent quadrants would be an unexpected search pattern.
Some recommendations while conducting testing include limiting the number of people testing each cohort of animals to 1 individual to reduce variability in handling and testing styles between experimenters, and carrying out testing at around the same time each day. Also, it has been suggested that mice receiving 2 trials per day learn nearly as quickly as those receiving 4 trials per day 15. It is essential that animals from different strains/backgrounds/treatment groups be represented in each testing group if more than one cohort needs to be run during a day. Finally, care must be taken to ensure the mice do not become hypothermic, as hypothermia can affect performance and is sex and background strain dependent 16. Though the inter-trial interval used here (20 min) should be sufficient to prevent hypothermia, other methods include adjusting the water temperature, placing the mice in a heated holding cage between trials, or some combination of the two as needed. However, it should be noted that water temperature can influence performance in both directions. For example, proestrous rats perform better under warm conditions (33 °C), whereas estrous rats perform better under cold conditions (19 °C) 17. Thus, care must be taken to control for water temperatures across experiments. Should hypothermia be a concern for a particular animal, the experimenter could hand dry that animal with a paper towel in the event that they fail to groom post-swim or give any indication of hypothermic behavior. Periodic assessment of body temperature can ensure the conditions are sufficient to prevent hypothermia.
Another procedural consideration pertains to visible platform training. Younger mice or another strain may need fewer visible platform training trials. A suggested guideline is to train until the control group has reached floor of performance during the final 3-6 consecutive training trials, providing the experimental group displays equivalent performance. If the experimental design includes both young and old mice, set the visible platform training duration for the group, which requires the highest number of training trials to reach floor of performance 5. In addition, the visible platform can be effective towards acclimatizing mice to the handling and test procedure and may obviate the need for pre-handling if the mice are not very young when first tested or human handling is not excessively aversive to the strain being tested.
Broad benefits of the Morris water maze include its relative insensitivity to motivational factors as compared to food-based tasks, its validity as a measure of hippocampal-dependent spatial navigation and reference memory, and its cross-species efficacy 15. A potential limitation of the technique is that, because this protocol is tailored for a specific background strain, it may not be effective with other animals or other background strains of mice. Additionally, as part of the protocol, attempts are made to create and strategically place identifiable cues throughout the mazing room. However, it is unclear what exact height is optimal when placing cues around the room. Thus, cues that are large and are distinguishable from one another are necessary for effective training.
In summary, optimizing the MWM task for use with a particular background strain and testing environment can significantly increase the dynamic range of the task, resulting in considerable time and cost savings.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
This work was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (Reed/Engler-Chiurazzi – U54GM104942), the National Institute for Neurological Disorders and Stroke (Ashe – R01NS33249, R01NS63249 and R01NS79374), CoBRE (Engler-Chiurazzi – P20GM109098), the Alzheimer's Association (Reed – NIRG-12-242187), a WVU Faculty Research Senate Grant (Reed), a WVU PSCOR grant (Reed), and internal funds from the WVU College of Medicine Dean’s Office (Engler-Chiurazzi). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIH or Alzheimer's Association.
Viewer Tracking software | Biobserve | This particular software is not a requirement – there are other tracking systems available | |
Pre-handling pool | Dimensions approximately 1 foot wide x 2 feet long x 1.5 feet deep | ||
Plastic beaker | 1 liter | ||
Scoop | |||
Small net | |||
Stopwatch | |||
White circular tub | |||
Non-toxic white tempera paint | Any color can paint can be used; must completely cover the hidden platform | ||
Platform | Color should contrast that of maze | ||
Curtain rod | |||
Curtains | |||
Mouse performance tracking software | |||
Circular tub | Uusally white in color; approximately 4 feet in diamater | ||
Platform | Painted same color as the water |