The dry-land Barnes maze is widely used to measure spatial navigation ability in response to mildly aversive stimuli. Over consecutive days, performance (e.g. latency to locate escape cage) of control subjects improves, indicative of normal learning and memory. Differences between rats and mice necessitate apparatus and methodology changes that are detailed here.
Spatial learning and memory of laboratory rodents is often assessed via navigational ability in mazes, most popular of which are the water and dry-land (Barnes) mazes. Improved performance over sessions or trials is thought to reflect learning and memory of the escape cage/platform location. Considered less stressful than water mazes, the Barnes maze is a relatively simple design of a circular platform top with several holes equally spaced around the perimeter edge. All but one of the holes are false-bottomed or blind-ending, while one leads to an escape cage. Mildly aversive stimuli (e.g. bright overhead lights) provide motivation to locate the escape cage. Latency to locate the escape cage can be measured during the session; however, additional endpoints typically require video recording. From those video recordings, use of automated tracking software can generate a variety of endpoints that are similar to those produced in water mazes (e.g. distance traveled, velocity/speed, time spent in the correct quadrant, time spent moving/resting, and confirmation of latency). Type of search strategy (i.e. random, serial, or direct) can be categorized as well. Barnes maze construction and testing methodologies can differ for small rodents, such as mice, and large rodents, such as rats. For example, while extra-maze cues are effective for rats, smaller wild rodents may require intra-maze cues with a visual barrier around the maze. Appropriate stimuli must be identified which motivate the rodent to locate the escape cage. Both Barnes and water mazes can be time consuming as 4-7 test trials are typically required to detect improved learning and memory performance (e.g. shorter latencies or path lengths to locate the escape platform or cage) and/or differences between experimental groups. Even so, the Barnes maze is a widely employed behavioral assessment measuring spatial navigational abilities and their potential disruption by genetic, neurobehavioral manipulations, or drug/ toxicant exposure.
Spatial learning and memory in laboratory rodents was first assessed with food-deprived rats that navigated a maze of alleyways to locate a food reinforcer1. Several decades later, a spatial reference memory system was proposed2. In contrast to working memory which refers to memory within a test session or trial, reference memory refers to memory across test sessions or trials and is more closely related to long-term memory.
Several types of mazes have been developed as noninvasive assessments of this hippocampal-dependent spatial learning and memory in small and large rodents (e.g. water maze, multiple T-maze, radial arm maze and dry-land mazes)3-6. Here, we focus on the circular platform or Barnes maze, first described in 1979 by Dr. Carol Barnes7. This maze has been used to test spatial navigational learning and memory in a wide range of rodent models, including rats (Rattus norvegicus), mice (Mus musculus), deer mice (Peromyscus maniculatus bairdii), California mice (Peromyscus californicus), and hystricomorph rodents (e.g. degus [Octodon degus])8-13. Other species assessed using the Barnes maze include American cockroaches (Periplaneta americana)14, corn snakes (Elaphe guttata guttata)15, squamate reptiles (e.g. side-blotched lizards [Uta stansburiana])16, and nonhuman primates (e.g. mouse lemurs [Microcebus murinus])17. In our labs, Barnes maze performance has been used as an index of neurotoxicity after developmental bisphenol A (BPA) or ethinyl estradiol (EE2) exposure9-1113. It is also commonly used for behavior phenotyping of various mouse strains18-21, assessment of aging effects7,22-28, and Alzheimer's Disease-related deficits in animal models3,29-33, as well as the effects of exercise and dietary, environmental, and metabolic alterations34-42.
A primary advantage of Barnes maze use is that it induces less stress in the subjects relative to water mazes, such as the Morris water maze43, although both can induce acute increases in plasma corticosterone concentrations in mice44. As a dry land maze, the Barnes maze may be more ethologically-relevant for terrestrial rodents45. Although water maze performance has been shown to be more sensitive to genetic alterations in mice3,46,47, Barnes maze performance is more sensitive to certain other alterations48,49. In rodent models where water maze use is not possible, the Barnes maze may provide a fine-tuned assessment of spatial memory retention31. The mildly aversive stimuli typically used in the Barnes maze (i.e. bright lights), however, may not provide sufficient motivation for the rodent to locate the escape cage45. Furthermore, rodents can learn that no punishment occurs if they do not enter the escape cage. Thus, instead of actively searching for the escape cage, some rodents actively explore the maze for long durations of each trial. As reviewed by Kennard and Woodruff-Pak24, this increased exploration will prolong the latency to locate the escape cage, path length, and increase the number of errors. Thus, measurement of multiple parameters, including latency, error rate, time spent in the correct and incorrect quadrants, velocity, time moving, time resting, and search strategy, may collectively provide a better indicator of each subject's spatial navigational learning and memory ability8-10. Additionally, performance can be measured as the latency to first locate the escape cage (primary measure) or the latency to enter the escape cage (total measure). Some have argued that primary measures of performance are a more accurate reflection of spatial learning than total measures50. Most studies, including the examples described here, use latency to enter the escape cage to determine error rate and search strategy. Further, some tracking software systems have a three point body detection system that can measure the frequencies of sniffing the correct vs. incorrect holes. Finally, the maze must be thoroughly cleaned with ethanol between trials to remove olfactory cues that could provide cues or prove distracting to subsequent animals.
Barnes maze designs vary but generally each has 12 or 20 potential escape holes, only one of which leads to the home or an escape cage. The escape cage may be situated either directly below the escape hole on the maze top (for mazes without walls) or built into the surrounding wall of the maze. The cues can vary in size from approximately 16.5 cm height or width (within the maze) to a horizontal line 21.6 cm in width placed from floor to ceiling of the room wall outside the maze. Figures 1-5 show examples of Barnes maze designs for Peromyscus species (Figure 1) and rats (Figures 2-5). Plugs or false bottoms must cover the nonescape holes to prevent the animal from falling out of the maze. Size of the test room can vary (~20 m2) but it must be large enough to provide ample room for the maze, habituating the animals to the room, accommodating a computer with video set-up (if used), and a place for the experimenter to sit at a distance (at least ~122 cm) from the maze apparatus such that their presence does not interfere with the animal's performance. Assignment of escape cage location should be balanced among treatment groups and sex. While the specific procedures described here do not include rotating the maze between trials to discourage use of the intra-maze odor cues, some studies incorporate this procedure50. In our procedures, the maze is wiped clean with ethanol between trials to eliminate odor cues.
In locating the escape cage, three types of search strategies have been defined (originally termed "patterns" by Barnes7): 1) random, operationally defined as localized searches of holes separated by paths crossing the maze center, 2) serial, defined as a systematic search of consecutive holes in a clockwise or counterclockwise direction, and 3) direct or spatial, defined as navigating directly to the correct quadrant without crossing the maze center more than once and with three or fewer errors. In general, with repeated testing, rodents typically progress through the search strategies in the order listed (random, serial, and direct)51. A probe trial without the escape cage may also be used as a further measure of memory50.
The protocol and representative results here were developed for two types of rodents (Peromyscus species- otherwise termed small rodents) and rats. While these general procedures may also hold for inbred and/or outbred mice (Mus musculus), other studies should be consulted on potential methodology differences for those latter species18-21.
1. Barnes Maze Procedure for Small Rodents
2. Barnes Maze Testing for Rats When a Tracking Program is Not Available
Initial Testing Day 1:
Days 2 through 7 Testing
3. Statistical Analyses for Barnes Maze Endpoints
Sexually mature male deer mice are dependent upon enhanced spatial navigational ability to locate potential breeding partners, which are widely disseminated throughout the environment. Both prenatal and adult exposure to testosterone are essential in organizing and activating this later adult male behavior53. As such, it was presumed that early exposure to endocrine disrupting compounds might disturb this later trait in males. To test this hypothesis, male and female deer mice were developmentally exposed via the maternal diet to several environmentally relevant doses of BPA in a phytoestrogen free diet, a positive estrogen control (ethinyl estradiol [EE2]) in a phytoestrogen free diet, or the base control phytoestrogen-free refined diet, and were assessed for Barnes maze performance as adults. Figure 1 shows the Barnes maze apparatus for this species. Males exposed to the two higher, but not the lowest, BPA doses demonstrated equivalent deficits in spatial learning, as manifested by prolonged latency, increased error rate, and an inability to convert to the direct search strategy over the trial period (Figures 7-9). However, females exposed to EE2 and the mid BPA dose, but not the other BPA doses exhibited masculinized patterns of spatial learning and memory (i.e. decreased latency and increased use of the direct search strategy)9,13.
In contrast to polygamous deer mice, their related cousins, monogamous male California mice, increase their reproductive success by pair-bonding and remaining in the territory with a single female and sharing in parenting responsibilities54,55. Therefore, spatial navigational ability has not been subject to strong evolutionary section in California mice. Consequently, the presumption was that early BPA and EE2 exposure would not target this behavior in California mice. In support of this hypothesis, developmental exposure to BPA or EE2 did not alter spatial navigational behaviors (latency, error rate, or conversion to the direct search strategy) in males or females, which demonstrated comparable responses across all treatment groups (Figures 10 and 11)10. Compared to control deer mice, control California mice did not decrease the number of errors made over the seven consecutive test days nor did the control California male mice increase their use of the direct search strategy. This may reflect a species difference in learning ability; however, it is possible that further refinement for assessment of visuo-spatial learning and memory testing is required for California mice.
The Barnes maze apparatus and associated hardware for rats are shown in Figures 2-5. This apparatus was used to assess spatial learning and memory of male and female Sprague-Dawley rats on postnatal days 47-51 (5 consecutive days, 1 trial/day). On the last day (i.e. day 5), the escape cage was moved 180° from its original location on days 1-4. These subjects had previously been assessed for the righting reflex and slant board behavior (preweaning), and play behavior, open field activity levels, and motor coordination. Their dams had consumed 3 small pieces of vanilla wafer onto which was dispensed 1 ml/kg body weight of water on gestational days 6-21. The subjects themselves were orally treated with 1 ml/kg body weight of water twice daily on postnatal days 1-21. At weaning, they were pair-housed with a same-sex sibling. However, only 1/sex/litter was assessed for Barnes maze performance. Figure 12 shows average latency to locate the escape cage for each sex on each of the 5 test days. Significant main effects of sex (p < 0.04) and session (p < 0.01) indicated shorter latencies in females and shorter latencies on days 2-5 relative to day 1. Others have also reported shorter latencies in female rats56; however, similar sex effects have not always been noted in our lab11. Thus, a consistent sex effect in rats is yet to be determined. Endpoints other than latency are not yet available; however, tracking software is in use in a similar study to examine error rate and search strategy in rats.
Figure 1. The Barnes maze apparatus for Peromyscus species. A) The intra-maze geometric cues (e.g. circle, square, triangle and star) are placed inside the maze wall every 90°, there are 12 escape holes placed every 30°, and the maze is surrounded by a black curtain (not shown). B) The maze top is placed on a polypropylene stand and elevated 100 cm above the floor. Click here to view larger image.
Figure 2. The Barnes maze apparatus for rats. The maze top and supporting stands can be seen with maze top diameter and height from floor shown. Numbers on the floor indicate hole numbers and allow the tester to place the escape cage in the designated location (the floor numbers cannot be seen by the subject). One of the extra-maze visual cues can be seen on the far wall (i.e. black vertical stripes). Click here to view larger image.
Figure 3. A closer view of the Barnes maze apparatus for rats. The white escape cage slides into grooves on the underside of the maze top. Similar grooves are located on the underside of the maze top for each perimeter hole. Click here to view larger image.
Figure 4. The center tube with sample subject identifying sheet on the top of the Barnes maze apparatus for rats. The cardboard cover lifts away to place the rat inside the tube and is then replaced. The handle on the center tube allows easy lifting to begin the trial. Click here to view larger image.
Figure 5. The escape cage for rats with dimensions. Small treads on the downward ramp provide traction for the rat when entering. Click here to view larger image.
Figure 6. Sample test sheet for Barnes maze. The handwritten information is recorded at the time of the test session while other information is generated beforehand. The columns are: 1) test order so that the animal cages may be arranged in order and the experimenter knows the order of testing, 2) session number (typically, 5-7 consecutive days), 3) animal ID, 4) sex (may not be necessary on the test sheet), 5) location of the escape cage for each subject, 6) as indicated by check marks in this column, animal numbers 106 and 131 did not find the escape cage within the allotted time, 7) the time of day for the beginning of each animal's trial, and 8) latency to locate the escape cage (in sec). At the bottom of the test sheet is ample space for the experimenter to record comments and notes, such as any noises or interruptions that might have occurred during the session. Click here to view larger image.
Figure 7. Effects of developmental exposure of male and female deer mice to BPA or EE2 on search strategy in Barnes maze. A) Example diagrams illustrating the three defined search strategies: random (top), serial (middle), and direct (bottom). B) Percentage of BPA, EE2 and control mice employing random (yellow), serial (green), or direct (black) search strategies across acquisition testing. CTL males utilized the direct search strategy more commonly over the 7 consecutive day test period than all other groups except low dose BPA males and EE2 females (all P values < 0.05). CTL = control; EE2 = ethinyl estradiol, BPA = bisphenol A. Adapted with permission from 13. Click here to view larger image.
Figure 8. Effects of developmental exposure of male and female deer mice to BPA or EE2 (same groups as in Figure 3) on latency to locate the escape cage in the Barnes maze. A) Males. B) Females. CTL males more rapidly located the correct escape cage, as exemplified by shorter latencies, than CTL females (P = 0.0103), EE2-exposed males (P < 0.0008), and upper and middle-dose BPA males (P = 0.03, P = 0.02, respectively). CTL males, however, showed similar responses as low dose BPA males and EE2 females (P's > 0.05). In contrast, EE2 females had decreased latency periods across the trial period than EE2 exposed males (P = 0.0013). Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Adapted with permission from Jasarevic et al.13 Click here to view larger image.
Figure 9. Developmental exposure of male and female deer mice to BPA and EE2 (same groups as in Figure 3) on escape errors. A) Males. B) Females. CTL males demonstrated approximately half of the number of errors or entries into incorrect holes compared to CTL females (P = 0.0002) and EE2 males (P = 0.02). Moreover, the CTL males committed fewer errors than upper dose BPA males (P = 0.02) but did not differ in error rate (P > 0.05) from either middle or low dose BPA males. On the other hand, EE2 females demonstrated a masculinized response such that this group possessed the same error rate as CTL males and reduced errors (P = 0.002) than EE2 males. Middle dose BPA-exposed females also showed less errors than low dose BPA and CTL females (P = 0.0005 and 0.01, respectively). Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Adapted with permission from Jasarevic et al.13 Click here to view larger image.
Figure 10. Search strategies of male and female California mice in Barnes maze testing. Search strategies are color-coded: random (yellow), serial (green), and direct (black). During the 7 day testing period, there were no significant effects of toxicant or sex on search strategy use for these animals. Adapted with permission from Williams et al.10 Click here to view larger image.
Figure 11. Latency to locate the escape cage and escape errors in Barnes maze testing for male (A & C) and female (B & D) California mice (same groups as in Figure 6). A and B) Latency. C and D) Escape errors. During the 7 day testing period, there were no significant effects of toxicant or sex on search strategy use for these animals. Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Adapted with permission from Williams et al.10 Click here to view larger image.
Figure 12. Latency to locate the escape cage for male and female Sprague-Dawley rats assessed on postnatal days 47-51 (1 trial/day). On the last day (day 5), the escape cage was moved 180° from its original location. Females exhibited significantly shorter latencies than males and latencies on days 2-5 were significantly shorter than latency on day 1. Data are presented as the mean ± SEM. Click here to view larger image.
Critical steps in Barnes maze testing procedures include: 1) providing the proper mildly aversive stimulus to motivate the animal to locate the escape cage, 2) ensuring uniform conditions are maintained across the animal trials (e.g. test time, testing personnel, external noise control, and other stimuli that might affect performance), 3) if trials are video recorded, optimizing and ensuring proper video recording and file back up, and 4) cleaning of the maze with 70% ethanol to remove olfactory cues between trials.
Identifying the best stimuli to motivate the subject to locate the escape cage can require some modifications and/or troubleshooting. The typical stimulus is bright illumination overhead. However, this may not be sufficient for some species. Although only noted anecdotally by us, rats that have been extensively behaviorally assessed (and therefore, extensively handled) seem less motivated under the standard Barnes maze conditions, likely because they become more docile and habituated to different apparatus and/or environments. Auditory stimuli (e.g. predator sounds) may be considered but this limits the ability to habituate simultaneously other animals to the testing room. Other stimuli that have been used successfully include overhead fans to direct air at the maze top57,58 or modifying the Barnes maze to be appetitive, instead of aversive56.
Extra-maze visual cues are the norm for Barnes maze testing with rats. In typical laboratory mouse species, it has been suggested that extra-maze cues may produce better results than intra-maze cues59,60. However, deer mice can successfully use intra-maze cues to locate the escape cage and do successfully convert over the testing period to use of the direct search strategy8,9. Moreover, an external wall prevents the animals from falling or jumping from the maze. As California mice are easier to handle and about 2-3x larger than deer mice, others have successfully tested this species in the Barnes maze without the use of a wall40,61. However, the maze in that case was smaller (65 cm in diameter) with 16 holes that were placed more inward (1.3 cm).
Methodologically, there are minor details which could affect the Barnes maze procedure and intrepretation of the results. The maze top for rodents is relatively large and the test room must be large enough to allow the tester to move freely around the maze. Placing the maze in a corner is not recommended as the tester must be able to move around the perimeter to retrieve the rat and place the escape cage in the appropriate location. Anxiety levels of the rodent, as evidenced by increased plasma corticosterone concentration44, are elevated during testing and extraneous stimuli could be exacerbating. Rodents typically freeze at sudden auditory stimuli and thus, it is important that the testing environment is not located in noisy area. Because this can be a lengthy assessment on a given day and across days, it can be challenging for the tester to remain attentive to the trial; however, direct attention to the subject's behavior is essential. For this reason and to avoid circadian effects on performance, it is optimal to test a limited number of animals for a select window of time (e.g. in the morning or afternoon) on a given day. Finally, the odor of ethanol may be aversive to the subject, although this has not been explicitly tested. Several escape cages and additional false bottoms are suggested so that the cages have time to air dry after being sprayed with ethanol.
The primary advantages of the Barnes maze are its ease of use relative to other maze types and the additional endpoints that can be obtained that may provide a more comprehensive assessment of experimentally-induced impairments. Additionally, this dry land maze may better recapitulate the natural environment for land-dwelling rodents. The multiday testing period could provide more robust evidence of altered performance, as evidenced by latency, error rate, and conversion over the course of testing from an inefficient search strategy (random or serial) to a direct search strategy.
Results from the Barnes maze can be verified with other tests of spatial navigation. In addition, it is important to establish that potential Barnes maze performance deficits are not a result of alterations in anxiety, activity, or motor abilities. Thus, results of anxiety and/or locomotor assessments, such as elevated plus maze or open field behavior, may determine if Barnes maze impairments reflect true alterations in spatial navigation. However, common murine tests of anxiety may not always be predictive of Barnes maze performance44. If true spatial navigation alterations are present, molecular, histopathological, electrophysiological, or synaptogenic changes might be apparent in the hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, or other cortical areas, as those brain regions appear to govern this learning and memory response62-64.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
The authors acknowledge Mr. Eldin Jašarević, Mr. Scott Williams, Mr. Roger W. Meissen, Sarah A. Johnson, Dr. R. Michael Roberts, Dr. Mark R. Ellersieck, and Dr. David C. Geary at the University of Missouri, and Mr. C. Delbert Law and the animal care staff at the National Center for Toxicological Research/FDA. This work was supported by an NIH Challenge Grant to Grant to CSR (RC1 ES018195), a Mizzou Advantage Grant to (CSR and DCG), a University of Missouri College of Veterinary Medicine faculty award (CSR), and protocol E7318 at the National Center for Toxicological Research/FDA.
NOTE: Those items that are for small rodents only are bolded. Those items that are for large rodents only are italicized. Items neither bolded nor italicized are for both. | |||
Barnes Maze platform with 12 or 20 escape holes every 30°. For rats, each hole is 10.5 cm in diameter and 4 cm from the maze top edge. For use with automated tracking programs, a black top for white rodents or a white top for pigmented rodents is needed. For mice and rats, this circular top is 95 and 122 cm in diameter, respectively. | US Plastics Corp, Lima, OH | 42625 | This is the top of the Barnes Maze and the surface that the rodent is placed upon. It can be constructed from a variety of materials (e.g. Plexiglas), but for endocrine disruptor work, polypropylene BPA-free material is optimal. One of the holes leads to the an escape cage; all other holes are blind-ending or false-bottomed. For the rat maze, small slides on the underside of the maze platform allow the escape cage and false bottoms to slide in. |
2" Polypropylene pipe plug (24) 2" 90° Black polypropylene elbow (12) 2" x 6" Polypropylene pipe nipple (1) |
US Plastics Corp, Lima, OH | 30724 32086 30712 |
These are only necessary for the small rodent (e.g. mouse) Barnes Maze. These adaptations are either blind-ending tubes/elbows or one of the tubes is connected to the pipe nipple that then leads to the escape cage. |
False bottoms for rat Barnes Maze | These were custom made of ABS plastic and vacuum molded for the rat maze apparatus. | ||
Circular aluminum wall/barrier (50 cm high) around the maze | Ace Hardware, Columbia, MO | In the case of small rodents (e.g. mice), this barrier prevents them from falling off the maze; the rat apparatus generally does not require this. The wall may not be needed for laboratory mice that are relatively tame. | |
Support stand for maze platform top | US Plastics Corp, Lima, OH | 42625 | The stand supports the maze platform top such that it is elevated above the floor (typically, 70-100 cm) to motivate the rodent to locate the escape cage. The stand can be constructed of any material. |
White noise | SleepMate Sound Conditioner, Marpac, Rocky Point, NC |
980A | Background noise may be used to block out peripheral acoustic cues that may confound Barnes Maze testing across trials and animals |
Light fixtures and 300-500 watt bulbs encased in aluminum shells. For example, Utilitech 500-watt halogen portable work lights. | Ace Hardware or Lowes | Bright lights provide a mildly aversive stimulus which motivate the rodent to locate the escape cage. The lights are generally suspended ~150 cm above the maze top. | |
Escape cage. For small rodents, this can be a polypropylene cage (27.8 x 7.5 x 13 cm). | Ancare, Bellmore, NY | N40 PP | The rat escape cage here was custom built and has a ramp leading into the escape cage. |
Opaque tube (rats only) (27 cm diameter; 23 cm height) with a piece of thick cardboard to cover the top. | The tube is placed in the center of the maze and the rat is placed into the tube from the top which is covered with the cardboard. A handle on the outside of the tube allows easier lifting of the tube, which then begins the trial. The tube can be constructed of any material, but should be opaque. | ||
High resolution video camera (e.g. Panasonic Digital Video Camera) | Panasonic, Secaucus, NJ | ICV19458 | The video camera is positioned overhead and records trials for later analysis. |
Extra- or intra-maze geometric cues made of high quality cardboard construction paper | Any office supply store, such as Staples. | These visual cues orient the animal within the maze environment, providing cues as to the spatial location of the escape cage; in rats, extra-maze cues on the walls work well, whereas in small rodents that require a wall around the maze, intra-maze cues must be used. | |
Black curtain to surround the maze (small rodents only) | Any fabric and crafts store, such as Jo-Ann Fabrics | A black curtain is used in small rodents (especially wild species, e.g. Peromyscus) to maintain attention within the maze confines. | |
70% ethanol | Fisher Scientific | BP2818-4 | After each trial, the maze top and escape cage are cleaned to eliminate potential odor cues for consecutively tested rodents. |
Tracking software program, such as Ethovision, and computer with appropriate video card and substantial (1 TB or more) hard-drive space. Alternatively, videos can be recorded directly to the computer for later analysis using a program such as Win TV (Hauppauge Computer Works, Inc.). | Noldus (Leesburg, VA) | Tracking software is required to analyze trials for latency to locate the escape cage, velocity, distance traveled, time spent resting, time spent moving, time spent in the correct versus incorrect quadrants, time spent around the escape hole, number of errors or entries into incorrect holes, and overall search strategy employed to find the escape cage. | |
External hard drives, such as Seagate or WD, with a minimum 1- 2 TB of memory | Any office supply store, such as Staples. | Videorecordings should be backed up in at least one separate location. | |
Videorecording program, e.g. WinTV program | Hauppauge Computer Works, Inc., Hauppauge, NY |
If tracking software is not available at the time of the testing, the trials should be video-recorded for later analysis |