The current methodology is designed to provide an ecologically relevant approach for measuring the veracity, length and quality of children’s true and false testimonies. Implications of the current methodology for future research and professionals who interview children will also be discussed.
A considerable amount of research has evaluated children’s lie-telling behaviors and skills1-2; however, limitations with the tasks used for eliciting false testimonies and interviewing children have restricted the generalizability of the findings. The primary aim of the current study is to provide an easy-to-administer and ecologically valid method for measuring the veracity and quality of school-aged children’s (ages 6-11) testimonies when they are asked to provide different types of true and false reports. Moreover, the methodology enables researchers to examine the social and developmental factors that could influence the credibility of a child’s testimony. In the current study, children will witness a theft, and are then asked to either falsely deny the transgression, falsely accuse a researcher of the theft, or tell the truth. Afterwards, children are to be interviewed by a second researcher using a thorough and ecologically valid interview protocol that requires children to provide closed-ended and free-recall responses about the events with the instigator (E1). Coders then evaluate the length and number of theft-related details the children give throughout the interview, as well as their ability to maintain their true and false reports. The representative results indicate that the truth and lie-telling conditions elicit the intended behaviors from the children. The open-ended interview questions encouraged children to provide free-recall information about their experiences with E1. Moreover, findings from the closed-ended questions suggest that children are significantly better at maintaining their lies with age, and when producing a false denial compared to a false accusation. Results from the current study can be used to develop a greater understanding of the characteristics of children’s true and false testimonies about crime, which can potentially benefit law enforcement, legal staff and professionals who interview children.
The primary goal of the present study is to provide an ecologically valid method for evaluating children's true and false testimonies in an experimental setting. Children's intentional false reports in police and forensic interviews have lowered the public's trust in the validity of child testimonies because of the potential negative consequences to the accused, accuser and criminal justice system3-6. A considerable amount of research has evaluated children's ability to falsely deny an event or transgression to protect someone7-12, yet considerably less is known about children's abilities to make a false accusation13-14. Even though children do make false denials and/or purposefully omit information in their testimonies, there have also been numerous real-life cases of children being persuaded to falsely accuse someone of committing a transgression, such as false allegations of abuse in custody battles5, 15-18. By having children willingly produce false denials and false accusations in an experimental setting, the current research methodology is designed to provide a stronger understanding of the types of false reports children can tell in their eyewitness testimonies.
Previous lie-telling research with children has generally involved a low-cost situation, whereby they were telling a lie about a non-threatening event, such as a broken toy9, 12, 14. Children who provide testimonies to police officers or forensic interviewers often disclose information about a high cost-event, such as witnessing a crime or experiencing abuse. Being asked to recall information about a non-threatening event may not promote deceitful behaviors in the same manner as when children actually witness a serious transgression or crime. For instance, children who witness a crime may experience symptoms of post-traumatic stress19-20; thus, they are often reluctant to discuss their potentially traumatic experience(s) with others21-22. To improve the understanding of children's ability to generate false reports in situations where they are asked to provide a testimony, the present study has children tell a truth or lie about an alleged theft that they may (or may not) have witnessed.
Past experimental research on children's lie-telling skills has typically used three-to-four follow-up questions to evaluate whether children's initial false claims were maintained throughout the interview10-11, 23-25. In addition, many of these interview protocols have relied on closed-ended questions where a child had to provide one-word responses, such as "Yes" or "No". Although such methodology does provide some insight into children's deceptive capabilities, the findings may not generalize to real-life settings where child witnesses are being questioned about an event. When providing a testimony to police officers or forensic interviewers, children often have to answer many questions that require them to provide both closed and open-ended responses; therefore, if a child is telling a lie, they will have to maintain it over multiple types of follow-up questions. To address this limitation, the present study will use a lengthier interview method to assess children's lie-telling skills, and the characteristics of their testimonies. The interview protocol is influenced by the Cognitive Interview (CI) and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) protocol, which are ecologically valid police interview techniques that are used for increasing the amount of information obtained from an eyewitness26-30. Rather than relying on a few closed-ended questions, the interview consists of two baselines, three open-ended, and seven closed-ended questions (refer to Appendix A). The larger number and variety of questions permits for the examination of age, gender and experimental condition-based differences in the length and types of information children are willing to disclose in their testimonies.
In order to appear convincing, lie-tellers often have to simultaneously manage their verbal and non-verbal behaviors, and assess the mental state of the lie-recipient10, 31-32. However, when interview questions require increased cognitive effort from the responder, lie-tellers are more likely than truth-tellers to make noticeable errors throughout their testimony33-34. Moreover, increasing the mental effort required by the responder actually discourages lie-telling35, as the cognitive load of telling a lie and responding convincingly to challenging and unexpected questions can be too cognitively taxing for some34, 36. For these reasons, police and forensic interviewers have been encouraged to use open-ended questions and prompts, such as those in the Cognitive Interview, to increase the cognitive effort required by the interviewee33-34, 35. In addition, these types of questions give honest responders multiple opportunities to elaborate on the information they previously disclosed, which can lead to more detailed and accurate testimonies30, 38-39. The current methodology can therefore provide data on children's true and false testimonies when they are asked generalizable interview questions and prompts that are intended to increase the cognitive load of the responder, and the amount of information they are willing to disclose about an event.
To overcome limitations with past studies, the current research methodology utilizes four-experimental conditions to evaluate the different types of true and false reports children are willing to provide regarding an alleged theft. In this study, children (ages 6-11) will witness an instigator (E1) discover a stranger's wallet with twenty dollars in it. Children are then asked by E1 to lie to an interviewer (E2) by either falsely denying a theft that occurred (False Denial condition), or by falsely accusing E1 of a theft that did not take place (False Accusation condition). In addition, some children will be asked to tell the truth about a theft that did (True Accusation condition) or did not take place (True Denial condition). Children are then to be interviewed by a second researcher (E2) about the events that took place with E1. The interview is to be videotaped, and later transcribed into written transcripts. Coders then record the number of words and wallet-related details disclosed by the children on the open-ended questions; responses on the closed-ended questions are used to evaluate children's ability to maintain their true and false reports.
NOTE: The following protocol was developed in accordance with the ethical standards approved by the McGill University Research Ethics Board. For all studies with children and involving deception, it is required to obtain Ethics Approval from the university or institution. Please note that different Ethics Boards may have different requirements.
1. Preparation for the Experiment
2. Participants
3. Filler Activities
4. Theft Situation
5. Setting the Experimental Condition
6. Interview
7. Concluding the Study
8. Preparation of Interview Transcripts
9. Coding Children's Open-ended Responses
10. Coding Children's Closed-ended Responses
Patterns of Lie-telling Behavior
Figure 1 shows the rate of children's lie-telling behavior in each experimental condition. As reported in Wyman, Taieb-Lachance, Foster, Crossman and Talwar (under review), child participants were more willing to tell a lie in the lie-telling conditions compared to the truth-conditions; however, no differences were found in the percentage of lie-tellers in the FD and FA conditions. In addition, no age or gender-related differences regarding children's willingness to lie were foundWyman, J., Taieb-Lachance, C., Foster, I., Crossman, A., & Talwar, V. False denials and false accusations in school-aged children's disclosures of a theft. Manuscript submitted for publication., (Under Review). Therefore, children's general willingness to lie in the FD (70%) and FA (73%) conditions and tell the truth in the TD (97%) and TA (93%) conditions suggests that each condition incited the intended behaviors from the participants. In accordance with past research8, 12, 14, 24, most children were willing to make a false denial to protect another. Nevertheless, due to the lack of experimental research on false accusations14, it is difficult to determine how the findings from the FA group relate to other research in the field.
Characteristics of Children's Testimonies
Table 1 displays the mean number of words and new information added by the children on each open-ended question. Although children did provide significantly more words and new details on the first open-ended question, the follow-up questions did encourage children to discuss more information regarding their experiences with E1. Children particularly demonstrated an increased willingness and ability to provide new information on the reverse-order question, which supports previous research regarding the value of this question30, 38-39. The current findings therefore show that the open-ended questions used in the current study encouraged children to provide qualitative information regarding their experiences with E1 continuously throughout the interview.
As reported in Wyman and colleagues (under review), children's abilities to maintain their lies improved significantly with age; however, no gender differences were found. Children in the FD condition had significantly higher lie-maintenance scores compared to those in the FA condition, while children's maintenance scores in the truth conditions corresponded to those in the FD condition. Children in the FA condition were therefore able to generate a false report on the open-ended questions, but they may have more difficulty maintaining that story when asked direct questions about the eventWyman, J., Taieb-Lachance, C., Foster, I., Crossman, A., & Talwar, V. False denials and false accusations in school-aged children's disclosures of a theft. Manuscript submitted for publication., (Under Review).
Figure 1: Children's Willingness to Lie within each Experimental Condition. The experimental conditions incited the appropriate responses from the participants as children (N = 103) were significantly more willing to provide false reports in the two lie-telling conditions, and give honest disclosures in the two truth conditions. No significant differences were found in children's willingness to provide an accusation versus a denial of the theft (true or false). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Response Length | Event Details | |
Free-Recall 1 | 87.11 (89.01) | 2.11 (4.51) |
Reverse-Order | 60.37 (58.89) | 0.79 (2.03) |
Free-Recall 2 | 43.67 (49.22) | 0.88 (2.25) |
Table 1: Children's Mean (SDs) Response Length and Number of Event Details on each Open-ended Question. Across the three open-ended questions, children (N = 68) provided free-recall disclosures regarding their experiences with E1, as well as some specific details about the theft. Although children provided the longest disclosure with the most event details on the first free-recall question, the reverse-order and second free-recall question did encourage children to talk more about their experiences with E1, and give some new information about the wallet situation.
The current methodology is designed to provide researchers with an ecologically valid method for evaluating different types of children's true and false reports. The representative findings suggest that the current methodology can encourage children to provide both false denials and false accusations about a high-cost event. In comparison to past studies that only examined children's false denials after witnessing a low-cost event (e.g., breaking a toy) 8, 9, 12, the current study can produce generalizable information about child testimonies as the participants will witness a high-cost event before telling different types of true and false reports. The use of a high-cost event may enable children to potentially experience some of the emotional discomfort, cognitive dissonance and/or mental effort associated with making intentional false statements. It is worth noting, however, that no child participants showed emotional distress at the end of the study, and no adverse incidents were reported during or after the study. This paradigm was carefully constructed to create an ecologically relevant crime situation, while still minimizing the risks to the child participants. Even though children will experience a high-cost event that can cause some discomfort, the current paradigm is not designed to cause serious distress that corresponds to the real life experiences children may testify about. Thus, the challenge for researchers is to create ecologically valid contexts that examine children's abilities to provide different types of true and false reports, while still upholding ethical standards and not causing harm to the participants.
Unlike past studies that only used a small number of closed-ended questions to interview the children10-11, 23-25, the present study utilizes an empirically supported interview structure that was designed to elicit cognitive load on the responders and encourage detailed disclosures. The interview includes a number of closed and open-ended questions, which will allow researchers to acquire comprehensive information regarding the quality of children's true and false testimonies (story maintenance), as well as the amount and type of information they are willing to disclose about a high-cost event. The representative results suggest that each open-ended question used in the interview encouraged children to provide detailed information about their experiences with E1. However, if some children were unwilling or unable to give thorough responses on the open-ended questions, the closed-ended questions could be used to gather more direct information from them. For these reasons, the ecologically valid interview structure will enable researchers to obtain generalizable data on the characteristics of children's true and false testimonies that can have direct implications to the legal and forensic fields.
There are some critical steps to this paradigm that can invalidate the results if they are not designed and/or administered properly. It is important that the two researchers conducting the study (E1 and E2) do not add or remove any information from their scripted dialogue. Children's behavior during the study, such as their willingness to lie and the type of information they disclose, can be influenced by what E1 and E2 say to them. Thus, each child should receive the same interview and information about the theft (based on their condition), and no information should be added or omitted. Also, child-parent interaction should be limited after the study begins. Previous research suggests that parents are very successful at persuading their children to tell a truth or lie12, 14, 43. A child's reaction to the theft and being asked to lie may be different if a parent witnesses the theft and/or discusses it with them before the interview. If a parent does interact with their child during the study, such as taking them to the washroom, the experimenters should ask the parents if any discussion of the study took place during that time. In the case that the experimenters believe that the parent influenced the child's behavior during the study, such as by discussing the wallet-situation with them, then that child's data should be excluded from the statistical analyses. Furthermore, children must not be aware of the true nature of the experiment until after the interview with E2. In order to acquire ecologically valid information on children's true and false testimonies, the participants must believe that everything they are experiencing in the study is real. If a child discloses that they are aware of the experimental nature of the study, the true denial condition should be used as the child will simply be disclosing information from memory about a non-threatening event (as no theft had taken place).
There are some limitations that should be considered when using this paradigm. First, it is unclear whether the current methodology can be used with clinical populations. For example, children with disruptive behavior disorders are more likely to have problems with aggression and impulse-control when encountered with an adverse situation44-46. In addition, children who have previous experiences with trauma may be reluctant to discuss the theft situation and/or react negatively to the study's procedures21-22. If researchers want to use clinical populations, pilot studies should be conducted to determine if any modifications or alternative research methods are needed before implementing this paradigm.
Second, the amount of time children spend with E1 and E2 should be controlled for. Past research suggests that children are more inclined to lie to an unfamiliar adult compared to someone they know25, and are more willing to lie to conceal the transgression of someone they know14. For this reason, the amount of time the child spends with E1 and E2, and their familiarity and relationship to them, can potentially influence their behavior during the study.
Third, the current study assesses how children produce true and false testimonies; nevertheless, it does not explain why children tell truths and lies. Researchers can therefore make modifications to the paradigm, such as by using questionnaires and/or debrief interviews at the end of the study, to better understand each child's reasoning for their behavior during the study, as well as their reflections of their experiences with the interview. Finally, some false accusations incorporate children being asked by the offender to accuse an innocent individual of committing the transgression47. In the current study, however, children are given permission by the instigator (E1) to falsely accuse E1 of the theft, rather than falsely accusing another without the consent of the accused. Thus, an alternative false accusation condition can include children being asked to falsely accuse an innocent third-party, which may generalize better to situations where children are asked to make false accusations.
Finally, the current study used an interview that took elements from the Cognitive Interview and the NICHD interview protocols. While the NICHD is regularly used with children and been found to be an effective interview method, the Cognitive Interview is not as commonly used with children although there is some evidence to suggest it is also effective with this population29. However, actual forensic interviews do not always follow either protocols. Future modifications could be made to the interview to reflect the most common forensic interviews or specific interview protocols.
The expected results from the current study can be beneficial to law enforcement, legal staff and professionals who interview children. The use of a more generalizable paradigm that encourages children to provide different types of false reports with a real-life interview structure can enable legal professionals to develop a greater understanding of the social and developmental characteristics of children's true and false testimonies. Moreover, by understanding how children produce different types of false reports, future researchers and legal professionals can develop strategies for encouraging honest disclosures. Past findings on the characteristics of adults' testimonies when using real-world interviews, such as the CI, have also been used to develop, and later evaluate the effectiveness of verbal and nonverbal lie-detection tools48-51; yet, very little research has evaluated the effectiveness of these lie-detection tools with children. Therefore, the data obtained from the experimental interviews with the children can potentially be used to assess the efficacy of different lie-detection tools, and develop new ones.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
This research was supported in part by a grant from the Social Science and Humanities Research Council. Also, a special thank you to the many families who participated in this study, and the volunteers and research staff who helped with the data collection.
Testing Room | N/A | N/A | The room where the rapport-building activities and theft occur. The room should have at least two chairs and two tables. One table will be used for the rapport-building activities and interactions between the instigator (E1) and the child participant. The second table will have the jacket and wallet placed on it. Other items, such as books, a labtop, flowers and office items can be placed on the second table to conceal the wallet. |
Common Room | N/A | N/A | Parents and other family members will remain in this room for the duration of the study. Comfortable chairs, a table, magazines, and age-appropriate toys will make the overall study experience more enjoyable for the family. |
Interview Room | N/A | N/A | The room that will be used to interview the children. The room should have a table, two-chairs, and hidden cameras to record the interview. |
Consent Form | N/A | N/A | Form that the parents compete prior to beginning the study. It should include a detailed explanation of the study, along with all the ethics considerations. Parents should sign this form before commencing the activities with the child. |
Demographics Form | N/A | N/A | A form that that provides any demographic information that is needed for the study, such as the race, income, religious background, and level of education of the family. |
Rapport-building Activities | N/A | N/A | Age-appropriate games and cognitive measures, such as a standardized verbal ability task. The rapport-building activities should take between 30 to 40 minutes. |
Jacket | N/A | N/A | A jacket that would realistically be worn according to the weather outside. |
Wallet | N/A | N/A | A gender-neutral wallet that includes fake identification cards, such as an old bus pass, and a twenty-dollar bill. |
Puzzle | N/A | N/A | A child-friendly puzzle that can be completed in 5 to 10 minutes. The current study utilized a puzzle that included 30 pieces. |
Interview Script | N/A | N/A | The interview script used in the current study. |
Hidden Cameras | N/A | N/A | Any camera that can easily be hidden from the child participants. The camera(s) should also be able to record individual HD videos for at least 30 minutes. |
Word Processing Program | N/A | N/A | Any word processing program that can be used to transcribe the videos. This program should also be able to count the number of words in a document. |
Statistical Analysis Program | N/A | N/A | Any statistical analysis program that can perform chi-square or logistic regressions for children’s willingness to lie data, and linear regressions on children’s maintenance scores. |