Detailed instructions are provided on how to train rats to voluntarily dive underwater through a 5 m long Plexiglas maze. Because the brains of rats have been very well characterized, voluntarily diving rats may help elucidate the central pathways of the mammalian diving response.
Underwater submergence produces autonomic changes that are observed in virtually all diving animals. This reflexly-induced response consists of apnea, a parasympathetically-induced bradycardia and a sympathetically-induced alteration of vascular resistance that maintains blood flow to the heart, brain and exercising muscles. While many of the metabolic and cardiorespiratory aspects of the diving response have been studied in marine animals, investigations of the central integrative aspects of this brainstem reflex have been relatively lacking. Because the physiology and neuroanatomy of the rat are well characterized, the rat can be used to help ascertain the central pathways of the mammalian diving response. Detailed instructions are provided on how to train rats to swim and voluntarily dive underwater through a 5 m long Plexiglas maze. Considerations regarding tank design and procedure room requirements are also given. The behavioral training is conducted in such a way as to reduce the stressfulness that could otherwise be associated with forced underwater submergence, thus minimizing activation of central stress pathways. The training procedures are not technically difficult, but they can be time-consuming. Since behavioral training of animals can only provide a model to be used with other experimental techniques, examples of how voluntarily diving rats have been used in conjunction with other physiological and neuroanatomical research techniques, and how the basic training procedures may need to be modified to accommodate these techniques, are also provided. These experiments show that voluntarily diving rats exhibit the same cardiorespiratory changes typically seen in other diving animals. The ease with which rats can be trained to voluntarily dive underwater, and the already available data from rats collected in other neurophysiological studies, makes voluntarily diving rats a good behavioral model to be used in studies investigating the central aspects of the mammalian diving response.
The diving response consists of a suite of autonomic reflexes seen in animals of all vertebrate classes 1. In response to submersion under water, this reflexly-induced response consists of apnea, bradycardia and an alteration of blood flow that maintains flow to the heart, brain and exercising muscles while limiting flow to viscera and non-exercising muscles 2. Many of the metabolic and cardiorespiratory aspects of the mammalian diving response have been well investigated 2,3, including those in humans 4,5. However, what has been relatively lacking, until recently, is investigation of the central integrative aspects of the diving response. What happens within the brainstem, and what is the neuronal step-by-step pathway, that connects afferent inputs to efferent outputs during this autonomic reflex? Answering these questions will require an appropriate animal model 6. An adage in comparative physiology, the Krogh principle 7, is that for every research question there is some animal of choice on which the problem can be most conveniently studied. A most appropriate animal for studying the central aspects of the diving response is the rat 6,8. In large part this is due to the fact that the brains of rats have been very well characterized, both anatomically and functionally, and many rat brain atlases are available 6. Additionally the rat is particularly useful in cardiorespiratory research, because the physiology of the rat is well known across all major organ systems, and the rat is well regarded as an animal model in systems biology 6. Finally, the laboratory rat is the domesticated version of the wild Rattus norvegicus, an animal that routinely swims and dives underwater 6. Based on these considerations, the rat is a good choice for studies investigating the central aspects of the mammalian diving response. In comparison, using marine animals to investigate the central aspects of the mammalian diving response would be much more difficult. This is due in large part to marine animals having comparatively large and non-uniformly sized brains, and the relative difficulty and high cost of housing these animals.
Rats have previously been used to investigate many aspects of the mammalian diving response, primarily in situations involving forced underwater submergence 9-12. However many studies in marine and aquatic animals have shown that there can be a differential response to diving based upon whether the submergence was forced or voluntary 2,13. Diving animals may show an extremely intense bradycardia during forced diving but a much less intense bradycardia during voluntary diving. The “stress” of forced submergence can significantly change the cardiovascular responses of diving in many animals 14. Small rodents such as muskrats also show a more intense diving response during forced submergence than during voluntary diving 15,16. Thus, if rats are to be used to investigate the central aspects of the mammalian diving response, investigators should be aware that a rat forcibly submerged underwater may produce a response different from that of a voluntarily diving rat.
The goal of this article is to provide detailed instructions on how to train rats to voluntarily dive underwater. These procedures are not technically difficult, but can be time-consuming. The training is conducted in such a way as to reduce the stressfulness that could otherwise be associated with forced underwater submergence. This voluntary diving technique should minimize activation of central stress pathways and thus better allow investigation of the central aspects of the diving response. By itself, training rats to voluntarily dive underwater generates no data that can be used to investigate the central aspects of the mammalian diving response. Therefore examples of how voluntarily diving rats have been used in conjunction with other physiological and neuroanatomical research techniques, and how the basic dive training may need to be modified to accommodate theses other techniques, are also provided.
NOTE: Experimental protocols described herein conducted at Midwestern University were approved by the Midwestern University IACUC.
1. Room Requirements
2. Diving Tank
3. Rat Allergen Considerations
4. Swim Training
5. Dive Training
NOTE: After the rats have learned to successfully negotiate swimming through the maze they are ready to start dive training.
6. Experimental Variations
NOTE: The basic experimental set-up and animal training have been described above. However, behavioral training only provides a model to be used with other experimental techniques to collect data of interest. Basic protocols are modified to investigate specific aspects of the diving response. Examples of these modifications, and some considerations for the collection of data using these physiological and neuroanatomical techniques, are given below.
Successful completion of the described swimming and diving training procedures can decrease the stress experienced by rats when diving under water. Blood corticosterone levels indicate that repetitive daily training decreases the stressfulness associated with voluntary diving, and trained rats find diving no more stressful than being handled daily by a human (Figure 2; 17). Conversely, rats not trained in the diving protocol find voluntary diving stressful (Figure 2; 17). Additionally, both trained and untrained rats find forced diving to be the most stressful (Figure 2; 17).
The cardiovascular responses from swimming, voluntary diving and forced diving rats have been recorded using implanted telemetry devices (Figure 3; 8,17-20) and trailing cannulae (Figure 4; 21-23). Immediately upon voluntary submersion, and within a single beat, heart rate decreases by 78% and mean arterial blood pressure decreases by 25% 17. These results show that voluntarily diving rats exhibit the same cardiorespiratory changes typically seen in other diving animals. Trailing arterial cannulae have been used to inject the muscarinic antagonist atropine, which eliminates the bradycardia associated with voluntary diving (Figure 4; 21), and to determine the distribution of cardiac output 22, including cerebral blood flow 23, during voluntary diving. Trailing cannulae have also been used to show that rats ignore increasing arterial hypoxemia and hypercapnia while they are submerged 18, and that pre-existing chemoreceptor drive does not have any effect on the cardiovascular responses to voluntary diving 21.
Neurons within laminae I and II of the ventral medullary dorsal horn (MDH) express Fos during voluntary diving, and these neurons may constitute the initial brainstem afferent relay of the diving response (Figure 5; 24). Important brainstem cardiorespiratory control areas, such as the caudal pressor area (CPA), nucleus tractus solitaries (NTS), rostral ventrolateral medulla (RVLM), and peribrachial regions, all show increased Fos labeling during voluntary diving compared with swimming 25. Neurons in chemosensitive regions of the brainstem express Fos after long duration forced dives 18.
Figure 1: Schematic of Diving Tank. A Plexiglas tank (100 x 60 x 15 cm) was used to create a simple maze consisting of five 1 m long channels. The tank was filled with 30 °C tap water, and rats were initially trained to negotiate the maze by swimming on the surface of the water, from the Start Area (top left) to the Finish Area (bottom right). The rats were then trained to dive through the maze, kept underwater by horizontal Plexiglas pieces placed 2-3 cm below the water surface. [This figure has been modified from 26]
Figure 2: Corticosterone measurements. Blood draws from rat tail veins were used to measure corticosterone concentrations (mean ± SE) from rats left in their cages (Naïve), rats handled for 10 min/day (Handled), rats trained to swim and dive (Trained), and rats that received no swim or dive training (Untrained). Corticosterone was measured after trained rats had completed their swim training (Left set of bars), after trained rats had completed their voluntary dive training (Center set of bars), and after trained rats had completed their forced dive training (Right set of bars). 1 indicates value is significantly greater than Naïve; 2 indicates value is significantly greater than Handled; 3 indicates value is significantly greater than Trained; * indicates that in Trained rats value during forced dive is significantly greater than during voluntary dive. [This figure has been modified from 17]
Figure 3: Arterial blood pressure traces from telemetric transmitters. Raw data traces showing pulsatile arterial blood pressure during swimming (left column), voluntary diving (middle column), and forced diving (right column) from rats trained to swim and dive through the maze (bottom row) and from rats that had not had the training procedure (top row). Diving underwater (both voluntary and forced submergence) produced an immediate bradycardia and slower onset increase in arterial pressure, whereas swimming on the surface of the water caused no such cardiovascular changes. Bars under traces indicate periods of submergence. Breaks in trace indicate periods when the telemetric signal was lost. [This figure has been modified from 17]
Figure 4: Atropine eliminates diving bradycardia. Original recordings of pulsatile arterial blood pressure of voluntarily diving rats (A) before and (B) after atropine pre-treatment. Traces were obtained using a trailing arterial cannula. Before atropine pre-treatment, arterial pressure decreased slightly upon submersion, but then increased to greater than pre-dive for the remainder of the dive. Heart rate was determined from adjacent pulse pressure intervals. Upon submersion there was an immediate and substantial bradycardia that was sustained for the duration of the dive. After parasympathetic blockade by atropine pre-treatment the bradycardia was eliminated. There was also an increase in arterial pressure during the dive. The bar under the trace indicates the period of submergence. [McCulloch, unpublished]
Figure 5: Fos labeling within the MDH. Photomicrographs of the trigeminal medullary dorsal horn (MDH) and the spinal trigeminal tract (sp5) in rats trained to dive underwater. (A) In a control rat that did not repetitively dive there is no Fos labeling. (B) In a swimming rat there is very little Fos label in the MDH (large arrowhead) or paratrigeminal nucleus (small arrow) within sp5. (C) In a diving rat there is more Fos labeling ventrally in both the MDH (large arrowhead) and paratrigeminal nucleus (small arrows) compared to the swimming and control rat. Insert in panel (A) indicates the rostral-caudal location of panels A-C. Scale bar in panel C is 100 μm. [This figure has been modified from 24]
Figure 6: Activated catecholaminergic neurons from diving rats. Photomicrographs show the medullas of a non-diving control rat (A, C, and E) and a voluntarily diving rat (B, D, and F). The brain tissue was immunohistologically processed for both Fos and tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), producing brown TH somas and black Fos nuclei. Open arrowheads identify single-labeled TH-positive neurons, while solid arrows identify Fos+TH double-labeled neurons. A1 neurons are identified in A and B. C1 neurons are identified in C and D. A5 neurons are identified in E and F. More Fos and TH double-labeled are seen in the A1, C1, and A5 regions of the diving rat than in the non-diving control rat. Calibration bar in E is for panels A-F, and is 250 μm. Calibration bar in inset in F is for all insets, and is 50 μm. [This figure has been modified from 26]
Rats in their feral form can and do exploit semi-aquatic environments, and will often dive underwater while foraging for food 6. Thus it is not too surprising that rats can be very easily trained to voluntarily dive underwater. The described training procedures may last up to 6 weeks, which will bring newly weaned rats to a body size used in most adult rat brain atlases (~300 g). Thus the brains from these trained animals will be more readily comparable to the anatomical structures identified in these atlases.
After being placed in the start area most rats will begin their underwater swim within 20 sec. However, occasionally a rat will take up to 5 min or more before initiating its voluntary dive. While it may be tempting at this stage to force the rats into the underwater tunnel, this generally should be avoided to prevent the rats from associating the water with a negative experience. Rodents can be stubborn and may initially refuse to dive during the training sessions, but once they realize the only way to exit the water is by completing the dive through the maze, they usually initiate their dives soon after being placed in the start area.
A critical aspect of the repetitious and methodical training procedures is that they are conducted in such a way to reduce the stress experienced by the rats. Letting rats explore their environment, especially while in the finishing area between trials, seems to further reduce stress. Rats will often re-enter the water to swim, and/or submerge their head underwater while still sitting on the finish area platform. This suggests that rats are not inherently water aversive. Additionally, while it is not uncommon for rats to produce fecal pellets during swimming or diving, or when waiting in the finish area, daily training results in fewer fecal pellets 17. In general the less stressed the rats are during the training, the fewer pellets they will produce. Any fecal pellets that are produced are removed from the water or finish area as soon as possible to keep the water relatively clean.
Rats may occasionally get bloody noses while swimming and diving, which may be a result of insufflation of water into the nasal passages. The appearance of blood may be due to osmotic stresses within the nasal mucosa. While in the finish area between trials the rats will groom themselves. As a consequence blood from the nose can get redistributed over the rats head and snout, giving the rats a slight reddish tinge, especially around the eyes, during the course of a training session. Also, an occasional rat may find diving stressful and/or have negative diving experiences (i.e., by turning around and getting lost while diving underwater (see note after 5.4.3. on how to prevent this from occurring)). In these rats porphyrin may appear in the corners of their eyes, signaling a stress response.
The size of the diving tank will to some degree determine the room requirements. The described tank is designed to have rats swim 2-3 cm under the surface of the water through a 5 m long Plexiglas maze to give an underwater swimming duration of 10-15 sec 17,19,24,26,27. Should an experiment be designed to measure responses from a longer duration dive, or from a deeper dive underwater, the tank may need to be re-designed. The room requirements could then also change to fit the dimensions of a re-designed diving tank. If there is no floor drain available in the procedure room, the water from the tank can be collected in a large container, such as a 60 gal garbage can, which can then be emptied elsewhere in a convenient manner.
The Fos technique can be utilized with other neuronal detection methods to further identify and characterize neurons that are part of the brainstem circuitry of the diving response. For instance, Fos detection in conjunction with tyrosine hydroxylase staining has identified catecholaminergic neurons in the A1, C1, A2, A5 and sub-coeruleus areas (Figure 6; 26), and globosa neurons within the lateral A7 area 26,27, that are activated during voluntary diving. Also, Fos detection in conjunction with the retrograde tracer Cholera toxin has identified the cell bodies of cardiac vagal motorneurons within the external formation of the nucleus ambiguus that are activated during voluntary diving 20.
Investigating the central nervous integration of the cardiorespiratory responses to diving is important for a number of reasons 6,8,28. The diving response enables animals, including humans, to remain submerged underwater without breathing for extended periods of time. The diving response represents a functional reorganization of brainstem homeostatic control, and demonstrates one of the most powerful patterns of autonomic reflexes observed in animals. The diving response may also be important clinically in humans as part of the trigemino-cardiac reflex, nasopharyngeal reflex, and/or sudden infant death syndrome. Finally, an understanding of the neuronal circuitry that exists within the brainstem of rats will help determine how cortical afferent signals can modify basic brainstem autonomic reflexes. All of these considerations make study of the central aspects of the mammalian diving response inherently worthwhile and interesting. Using the described procedures to train rats to voluntarily dive underwater will allow better investigation of the central aspects of the mammalian diving response than will the use of forced dived animals. This is because the training procedures as described 1) reduce activation of CNS stress circuitry, and 2) do not activate CNS reward circuitry because external rewards are not used.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
Research supported by funding from the Midwestern University Office of Research and Sponsored Programs. Thanks also to the Midwestern University Animal Facility and Erik Warren.
Name of Material/ Equipment | Company | Catalog Number | Comments/Description |
1 in internal diameter tubing | Fisher | 14-169-63 | Used to fill or drain tank |
N95 mask – Moldex #2300N Series | Fisher | 19-003-246D | Used to limit inhalation of rat allergens |
Plexiglas rodent restraint device (Economy flat bottomed restrainer) | Braintree | FB-M/L | For forced dives |
Telemetric transmitters | DSI | Model PA-C40 (270-0040-008) | Used to transmit pulsatile arterial blood pressure |
Hand-held antenna wand | DSI | Model RLA 3000 (272-5007) | Used to ensure radio antenna is near to transmitter while rat is negotiating underwater maze |
Intramedic PE50, 0.023" ID | Fisher | 14-170-12B | Used as trailing arterial cannula |