Inducing Emotions

JoVE Science Education
Social Psychology
Zum Anzeigen dieser Inhalte ist ein JoVE-Abonnement erforderlich.  Melden Sie sich an oder starten Sie Ihre kostenlose Testversion.
JoVE Science Education Social Psychology
Inducing Emotions

14,891 Views

10:22 min

April 30, 2023

Übersicht

Source: William Brady & Jay Van Bavel—New York University

Psychologists have long known that people behave differently in good moods versus bad moods, and this general principle extends to consumer behavior. Economists, as well, have come to appreciate that an individual’s financial decisions are not solely the result of extensive cost-benefit calculations; other factors like emotion are at play. Further, incidental emotions affect the behavior of buyers and sellers even though they are unrelated to the transaction at hand. While earlier research focused on the impact of global feelings (positive-negative), more recent research examines more specific emotions (e.g., anger and fear). In consumer settings, research shows that anger triggers greater risk-seeking behavior among buyers and sellers and that fear triggers the opposite, i.e., conservative behavior.  

The following experiment tests how two specific negative emotions—disgust and sadness—influence people’s financial valuation of objects.1 The experiment examines the relationships between induced emotional states (disgust and sadness) and the endowment effect. Inherent in this experiment is a common technique for inducing specific emotions in a laboratory setting. Once the emotions are created, they can then be implemented in a number of experimental conditions.

Grundsätze

The endowment effect is the tendency for people to overvalue objects they own. As a seller, people demand higher prices for objects they own than they would be willing to pay themselves, as buyers.

Incidental emotions are feelings that a consumer or seller carries with them to a potential financial transaction but are not caused by the potential transaction. For example, a consumer may be contemplating the purchase of a new TV while feeling sadness over an earlier argument with their spouse.

Verfahren

1. Participant Recruitment

  1. Conduct a power analysis and recruit a sufficient number of participants with a diversity of age and gender.
  2. Randomly assign participants in the experimental and control conditions.

2. Data Collection

  1. Seat participants in individual cubicles, each equipped with a computer and headphones.
  2. Tell participants that they were going to complete two separate studies and give participants two packets of material, one for each study.
    1. In truth, the first portion of the study (i.e., the materials in the first packet) is used to induce the desired emotion without letting the participant know that the two supposedly separate studies are related.
  3. Manipulate the first independent variable at this time. Participants are randomly assigned to one of two conditions:
    1. In the selling condition, give them a highlighter set and instruct that they will use it in the subsequent study.
    2. In the choice condition, do not give them a highlighter set.
  4. Induce specific emotions.
    1. Instruct each participant to complete a self-report questionnaire called the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule, which assesses their baseline emotions.
      1. This baseline measure is used to ensure that pre-existing emotions  are not attributed to the emotion manipulation.
    2. Manipulate the second independent variable using a video clip meant to induce emotions. Randomly assign participants to watch one of three 4-min clips.
      1. In the sadness condition, participants watch a 4-min scene from a sad movie (involving the death of a boy’s mentor).
        1. Ask the participants to imagine what it would feel like if they were personally in the situation shown in the clip and to write down their feelings.
      2. In the disgust condition, participants watch a disgusting 4-min scene from a film (involving a man using an unsanitary toilet).
        1. Ask the participants to imagine what it would feel like if they were personally in the situation shown in the clip and to write down their feelings.
      3. In the neutral condition, participants watch a 4-min scene from a documentary about nature (involving fish in the Great Barrier Reef).
        1. Ask the participants to simply write about their daily activities.
  5. Tell participants that they now have to complete the second study next and to open the second packet of instructions.
    1. Participants who were previously assigned to the selling condition are presented with a list of 28 prices, ranging from $0.50 to $14, and are asked to indicate, for each price, whether they would prefer to sell the highlighter set or keep it.
    2. Participants in the choice condition are shown the highlighter set at this time and then given the same list of 28 prices as participants in the selling condition. They are asked if they would rather have the amount of money listed or the highlighter set.
  6. Emotion induction manipulation check: This is designed to show that emotions were manipulated as intended.
    1. Present participants with a list of 27 affective states and ask them to indicate whether they feel each affective state listed on a scale, ranging from 0 (do not experience emotion at all) to 8 (experience the emotion more strongly than ever before).
      1. Among these affective states are five key items that relate to sadness (e.g., blue, downhearted, and sad) and disgust (e.g., disgust and repulsed).
  7. Fully debrief participants.

3. Data Analysis

  1. Examine the average cost at which participants would sell their highlighter set (selling condition) or choose the highlighter set (choice condition).
  2. Compare these means for those in each of the emotion conditions and analyze using planned 2 x 2 contrasts in ANOVA as in the original paper, or ideally, using a 2 x 3 ANOVA.

People behave differently based on their mood, and as it turns out, this principle extends to the consumer market, where financial decisions are not just the result of cost-benefit calculations; other factors—like emotion—are at play when valuing particular items.

For instance, while an individual is shopping online for a new instrument, his friend calls and they get into a heated discussion. The resulting state of anger triggers greater risk-seeking behavior, and he purchases an unreasonably high-priced item.

His roommate—as a witness to the ordeal—now feels afraid, and in this different negative state of fear, is more uncertain about the product and contrarily thinks that it’s priced too high to buy.

In such cases, anger and fear are known as incidental emotions—feelings that consumers carry with them into potential financial transactions but are unrelated to the potential purchases at hand.

This video demonstrates how to induce incidental emotions—specifically disgust and sadness—and test their influences on individuals’ financial valuations of objects—just how much they would pay or want to be paid for something.

This experiment consists of two parts: in Phase 1, participants are assigned to either a selling or choice condition and then watch videos to induce specific emotions. In Phase 2, they are asked how much they would sell or buy a particular object for.

During Phase 1, half of the participants—those in the selling condition—are endowed with a highlighter set and instructed that they will use it in a subsequent study. The other half in the choice condition does not receive it.

To measure pre-existing feelings, each participant is first asked to complete a self-reported questionnaire—the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. Following this baseline assessment, participants are randomly assigned to watch one of three, 4-min video clips to induce emotions.

In the sadness condition, the chosen scene is from a depressing movie—one that involves a significant character dying. For the second disgust group, a situation that is repulsive, like entering an unsanitary room, is portrayed. Lastly, in the neutral condition, a nature documentary is shown.

During this observation phase, participants in the experimental groups—sadness and disgust—are asked to imagine what it would feel like if they were personally in the situation shown in the clip and to write down their feelings. Those in the control group—watching the neutral video—are instructed to simply write about their daily activities.

For Phase 2, participants who were assigned to the selling condition are presented with a list of 28 prices, ranging from $0.50 to $14, and are asked to indicate, at each price, whether they would prefer to sell the highlighter set or keep it.

Those in the choice condition are shown the markers and then given the same list of 28 prices. In this case, they are asked if they would rather have the amount of money listed or the highlighter set.

Finally, to see if the expected emotions were induced, participants are once again given a list of affective states, including blue, downhearted, and sad, as well as disgusted and repulsed. They are asked to rate whether they feel any of them on a scale of 0 (do not experience emotion at all) to 8 (experience the emotion more strongly than ever before).

In this experiment, the dependent variable is the average cost at which participants would either sell their highlighter set or choose to buy it. Averages are calculated for each emotion and buy/sell conditions.

Based on previous work, disgust evokes the need to expel and avoid acquiring anything new. Therefore, it is predicted that participants in this emotional state will reduce both selling prices—amongst those who possessed the item—and buying prices—for those who did not have ownership—relative to the neutral conditions.

Such findings would eliminate the endowment effect—the tendency for people to overvalue objects that they own.

Conversely, sadness triggers the psychological need to change one’s circumstances. Thus, it’s hypothesized that individuals feeling down will decrease selling prices to get rid of what they have, whereas the others will increase their buying price to acquire something new. In this case, the endowment effect is reversed.

Prior to the experiment, conduct a power analysis to recruit a sufficient number of participants for this 3 x 2 between-subjects design.

Then, arrange two packets of material for each individual: In the first one, insert a questionnaire called the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule and a sheet to write down their thoughts; in the second, enclose a highlighter set for those randomly assigned to the selling condition, a price list, and another survey on affective states.

To begin, escort a participant into the testing room near a computer and headphones. Inform them that they are going to complete two separate studies, and hand out the materials for both.

Now, instruct participants to open their envelopes. Notice that those randomly assigned to the selling condition were given a highlighter set to use in the 2nd study, whereas those in the choice condition were not provided with one.

As a baseline self-assessment, ask each participant to fill out the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule questionnaire. After the form is completed, play one of three, 4-min video clips.

For participants watching the clips of sadness or disgust, ask them to imagine what it would feel like if they were personally in the situation shown in the clip and to write down their feelings. For those watching the neutral clip, simply have them write about their daily activities.

To start the second phase, instruct participants to retrieve the list of 28 price amounts, ranging from $0.50 cents to $14.

For those assigned to the selling condition, ask them to indicate, for each price, whether they prefer to sell the highlighter set or keep it. On the other hand, for those in the choice condition, have them specify if they would rather obtain the listed amount of money or the highlighter set.

Afterwards, direct each participant to complete the final list of affective states, in which they should remark on whether or not they experienced the emotion listed on a scale from 0 to 8. To conclude, debrief them about the details of the study.

To visualize the data, plot the average cost at which participants would sell their highlighter set or choose to obtain it across the three emotional manipulations of neutral, disgust, and sadness.

Compared to the neutral conditions, when sadness was induced, participants in the selling condition reported decreased prices, but those in the choice group reported increased amounts.

Thus, the endowment effect was reversed, suggesting that sadness evokes change—whereby individuals undervalue the objects they own and would pay more to possess something new.

However, relative to the neutral controls, disgust-induced participants indicated reduced prices in both the selling and choice conditions. In this case, the endowment effect was eliminated, implying that disgust conjures a need to expel or acquire any new objects. Collectively, even though both emotions were negative, they triggered different economic decisions.

Now that you are familiar with how to induce emotions in an experimental setting, let’s look at the implications for marketing and advertising and how businesses can alter what we value.

If people are willing to spend more for an item when they are feeling down, sadly, a salesman could get away with pushing higher-priced goods in such situations. In contrast, people are less willing to spend money when disgusted. Thus, marketers should avoid inducing this emotion in buyers.

Moreover, businesses are notorious for pumping irresistible smells into the air. This powerful manuever is intended to rapidly trigger pleasant emotions, which lure children and adults alike to purchase items they had no intention of buying prior to detecting the irresistible scents.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on inducing emotions. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design, conduct, and analyze an experiment to study how specific incidental feelings impact object valuation, as well as how emotional states can be applied to advertising and marketing.

Thanks for watching!

Ergebnisse

The data (Figure 1) show support for the idea that compared to the neutral condition, sadness decreased the prices for those in the selling condition, but increased prices for those in the choice condition. However, compared to the neutral condition, disgust reduced prices for participants in both the selling and choice conditions.

Figure 1
Figure 1: Results showing average selling and choice prices for neutral participants, disgusted participants and sad participants. Price is on the y-axis and and experimental condition is on the x-axis.

Applications and Summary

Although they are both negative emotions, disgust and sadness trigger different economic behavior. Disgust triggers the psychological need to expel, thus reducing both buying and selling prices. Conversely, sadness triggers the psychological need to change one’s circumstances, thus increasing buying prices and decreasing selling prices. With disgust, the endowment effect is eliminated, whereas with sadness the effect is reversed.

Emotion and cognition influence one another. Our appraisals of situations can drive emotions, and our emotional states can influence the way we think. Moreover, decisions are rarely made in an emotional vacuum; often, people are experiencing strong emotions before, during, and after they make decisions. Even our calculations of economic value are influenced by our emotional states. For example, the sadness from going through a divorce could actually alter our consumer-spending.

This study has clear implications for advertising and marketing. These findings suggest that people are willing to spend more for an item when they are sad, which could be leveraged by producing sadness-inducing marketing and by pricing items higher in situations in which consumers are likely to be sad. People are less willing to spend money when disgusted, likely because they value objects less (perhaps due to a sense of contamination). Thus, marketers should avoid inducing disgust in consumers. This is an important insight, given that mild comic disgust is quite common in advertising. Conversely, someone seeking to buy something might want to make the seller feel disgust, as they may be willing to sell the item for a lower price.

Referenzen

  1. Lerner, J. S., Small, D. A., & Lowenstein, G. (2004). Heart strings and purse strings: Carryover Effects of emotions on economic decisions. Psychological Science, 15, 337-341.

Transkript

People behave differently based on their mood, and as it turns out, this principle extends to the consumer market, where financial decisions are not just the result of cost-benefit calculations; other factors—like emotion—are at play when valuing particular items.

For instance, while an individual is shopping online for a new instrument, his friend calls and they get into a heated discussion. The resulting state of anger triggers greater risk-seeking behavior, and he purchases an unreasonably high-priced item.

His roommate—as a witness to the ordeal—now feels afraid, and in this different negative state of fear, is more uncertain about the product and contrarily thinks that it’s priced too high to buy.

In such cases, anger and fear are known as incidental emotions—feelings that consumers carry with them into potential financial transactions but are unrelated to the potential purchases at hand.

This video demonstrates how to induce incidental emotions—specifically disgust and sadness—and test their influences on individuals’ financial valuations of objects—just how much they would pay or want to be paid for something.

This experiment consists of two parts: in Phase 1, participants are assigned to either a selling or choice condition and then watch videos to induce specific emotions. In Phase 2, they are asked how much they would sell or buy a particular object for.

During Phase 1, half of the participants—those in the selling condition—are endowed with a highlighter set and instructed that they will use it in a subsequent study. The other half in the choice condition does not receive it.

To measure pre-existing feelings, each participant is first asked to complete a self-reported questionnaire—the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule. Following this baseline assessment, participants are randomly assigned to watch one of three, 4-min video clips to induce emotions.

In the sadness condition, the chosen scene is from a depressing movie—one that involves a significant character dying. For the second disgust group, a situation that is repulsive, like entering an unsanitary room, is portrayed. Lastly, in the neutral condition, a nature documentary is shown.

During this observation phase, participants in the experimental groups—sadness and disgust—are asked to imagine what it would feel like if they were personally in the situation shown in the clip and to write down their feelings. Those in the control group—watching the neutral video—are instructed to simply write about their daily activities.

For Phase 2, participants who were assigned to the selling condition are presented with a list of 28 prices, ranging from $0.50 to $14, and are asked to indicate, at each price, whether they would prefer to sell the highlighter set or keep it.

Those in the choice condition are shown the markers and then given the same list of 28 prices. In this case, they are asked if they would rather have the amount of money listed or the highlighter set.

Finally, to see if the expected emotions were induced, participants are once again given a list of affective states, including blue, downhearted, and sad, as well as disgusted and repulsed. They are asked to rate whether they feel any of them on a scale of 0 (do not experience emotion at all) to 8 (experience the emotion more strongly than ever before).

In this experiment, the dependent variable is the average cost at which participants would either sell their highlighter set or choose to buy it. Averages are calculated for each emotion and buy/sell conditions.

Based on previous work, disgust evokes the need to expel and avoid acquiring anything new. Therefore, it is predicted that participants in this emotional state will reduce both selling prices—amongst those who possessed the item—and buying prices—for those who did not have ownership—relative to the neutral conditions.

Such findings would eliminate the endowment effect—the tendency for people to overvalue objects that they own.

Conversely, sadness triggers the psychological need to change one’s circumstances. Thus, it’s hypothesized that individuals feeling down will decrease selling prices to get rid of what they have, whereas the others will increase their buying price to acquire something new. In this case, the endowment effect is reversed.

Prior to the experiment, conduct a power analysis to recruit a sufficient number of participants for this 3 x 2 between-subjects design.

Then, arrange two packets of material for each individual: In the first one, insert a questionnaire called the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule and a sheet to write down their thoughts; in the second, enclose a highlighter set for those randomly assigned to the selling condition, a price list, and another survey on affective states.

To begin, escort a participant into the testing room near a computer and headphones. Inform them that they are going to complete two separate studies, and hand out the materials for both.

Now, instruct participants to open their envelopes. Notice that those randomly assigned to the selling condition were given a highlighter set to use in the 2nd study, whereas those in the choice condition were not provided with one.

As a baseline self-assessment, ask each participant to fill out the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule questionnaire. After the form is completed, play one of three, 4-min video clips.

For participants watching the clips of sadness or disgust, ask them to imagine what it would feel like if they were personally in the situation shown in the clip and to write down their feelings. For those watching the neutral clip, simply have them write about their daily activities.

To start the second phase, instruct participants to retrieve the list of 28 price amounts, ranging from $0.50 cents to $14.

For those assigned to the selling condition, ask them to indicate, for each price, whether they prefer to sell the highlighter set or keep it. On the other hand, for those in the choice condition, have them specify if they would rather obtain the listed amount of money or the highlighter set.

Afterwards, direct each participant to complete the final list of affective states, in which they should remark on whether or not they experienced the emotion listed on a scale from 0 to 8. To conclude, debrief them about the details of the study.

To visualize the data, plot the average cost at which participants would sell their highlighter set or choose to obtain it across the three emotional manipulations of neutral, disgust, and sadness.

Compared to the neutral conditions, when sadness was induced, participants in the selling condition reported decreased prices, but those in the choice group reported increased amounts.

Thus, the endowment effect was reversed, suggesting that sadness evokes change—whereby individuals undervalue the objects they own and would pay more to possess something new.

However, relative to the neutral controls, disgust-induced participants indicated reduced prices in both the selling and choice conditions. In this case, the endowment effect was eliminated, implying that disgust conjures a need to expel or acquire any new objects. Collectively, even though both emotions were negative, they triggered different economic decisions.

Now that you are familiar with how to induce emotions in an experimental setting, let’s look at the implications for marketing and advertising and how businesses can alter what we value.

If people are willing to spend more for an item when they are feeling down, sadly, a salesman could get away with pushing higher-priced goods in such situations. In contrast, people are less willing to spend money when disgusted. Thus, marketers should avoid inducing this emotion in buyers.

Moreover, businesses are notorious for pumping irresistible smells into the air. This powerful manuever is intended to rapidly trigger pleasant emotions, which lure children and adults alike to purchase items they had no intention of buying prior to detecting the irresistible scents.

You’ve just watched JoVE’s video on inducing emotions. Now you should have a good understanding of how to design, conduct, and analyze an experiment to study how specific incidental feelings impact object valuation, as well as how emotional states can be applied to advertising and marketing.

Thanks for watching!