Science Education
>

Using Cholesky Decomposition to Explore Individual Differences in Longitudinal Relations between Reading Skills

PREPARAÇÃO DO INSTRUTOR
CONCEITOS
PROTOCOLO DO ALUNO
JoVE Journal
Comportamento
É necessária uma assinatura da JoVE para visualizar este conteúdo.  Faça login ou comece sua avaliação gratuita.
JoVE Journal Comportamento
Using Cholesky Decomposition to Explore Individual Differences in Longitudinal Relations between Reading Skills

The steps below describe the process of estimating individual differences underlying longitudinal associations between elementary and middle school reading skills into (A) genetic, (C) shared environmental, and (E) non-shared environmental factors using a statistical modeling program, word processor, and software with a graphical user interface (GUI). This study has been approved by the Institutional Review Board at Florida State University.

1. Preparing data for the statistical modeling program

  1. Prepare the data in a format that can be read by the statistical modeling program of choice. Popular statistical modeling programs include Mx, OpenMx in the platform R, and MPlus9. Mx can read data files in .vl or .dat data formats, OpenMx in any data format, and Mplus in a .dat data format. The example demonstrated here is executed in the program MPlus9.
    NOTE: A sample data file in a .dat format for six randomly chosen participants is available in supplemental files. Variables used in a sample data file reflect variables used in the input coding file.

2. Reading data into the statistical modeling program, running the script, and estimating the effects

  1. Open the statistical modeling program.
  2. Locate the relevant data file to be read into the statistical modeling program by typing “File is [insert location of your data file on your computer]”.
  3. Click on the icon RUN on the ribbon of the statistical modeling program to obtain estimates for genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental influences from the multivariate Cholesky decomposition method. The annotated input script for the multivariate Cholesky decomposition model for four time points as well as its output using MPlus can be found in supplemental coding files.
  4. Once the statistical modeling program generates estimates for genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental influences, locate the estimates in the output file under stx11 for path a11, stx21 for path a21, …, sty11 for path c11, sty21 for path c21, …, stz11 for path e11, stz21 for path e21, etc.

3. Creating a table with generated estimates

  1. Open the word processor.
  2. Copy the generated estimates into a table in a word processor. The table can be created in a format as indicated in Figure 3. For example, in this case, the estimates for the paths a11, a21, a31, and a41 have values of 0.60, 0.24, 0.63, and 0.18, respectively.

Figure 3
Figure 3: Multivariate Cholesky decomposition modeling standardized path estimates of genetic and environmental influences. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

4. Plotting genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental influences

  1. Open the software with a GUI.
  2. Enter the estimates from the created table into cells F3-F16, G4-G16, H5-H16, and I6-I16. A screenshot from the software with a GUI is depicted in Figure 4.

Figure 4
Figure 4: Entering of estimates into the software with a GUI. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. Calculate the variance of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental influences by squaring the estimates in cells F3-F16, G4-G16, H5-H16, and I6-I16. Type the squared values in cells J3-J16, K4-K16, L5-L16, and M6-M16.
  2. Calculate the percentage variance by multiplying values in cells J3-J16, K4-K16, L5-L16, and M6-M16 by 100. Type the percentage values in cells N3-N16, O4-O16, P5-P16, and Q6-Q16. Steps 4.3 and 4.4 are depicted in Figure 5.

Figure 5
Figure 5: Illustration of steps 4.3 and 4.4. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. Calculate the extent to which genetic influences carry over (overlap) from elementary to middle school.
    1. In cell R3, type “0”.
    2. In cell R4, type “=N4”. This is the extent to which genetic influences from the first time point carry over to the second time point. In this case, it indicates genetic influences from letter naming fluency in kindergarten carrying over to phoneme segmentation fluency in kindergarten.
    3. In cell R5, type “= N5+O5”. This is the degree to which genetic influences from the first two time points carry over to the third time point. In this case, it indicates genetic influences from letter naming fluency in kindergarten and phoneme segmentation fluency in kindergarten carrying over to word-level reading skills in grade 1.
    4. In cell R6, type “= N6+O6+P6”. This is the extent to which genetic influences from the first three time points carry over to the fourth time point. In this case, it indicates genetic influences from letter naming fluency in kindergarten, phoneme segmentation fluency in kindergarten, and word-level reading skills in grade 1 carrying over to reading comprehension in grade 7.
  2. Calculate the extent to which shared environmental and non-shared environmental influences carry over (overlap) from elementary to middle school much the way as in step 4.5.
  3. Calculate the extent to which unique genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental factors come online at each particular time point (i.e., grade).
    1. Copy the percentages from cells N3, O4, P5, and Q6 into cells S3, S4, S5, and S6, respectively, to obtain the extent to which unique genetic factors come online at each grade.
    2. Copy the percentages from cells N8, O9, P10, and Q11 into cells U3, U4, U5, and U6, respectively, to obtain the extent to which unique shared environmental factors come online at each grade.
    3. Copy the percentages from cells N13, O14, P15, and Q16 into cells W3, W4, W5, and W6, respectively, to obtain the extent to which unique non-shared environmental factors come online at each grade.
  4. To ensure all calculations are correct, the values in cells R3-W3, R4-W4, R5-W5, and R6-W6 should each add up to 100. Steps 4.5–4.7 are depicted in Figure 6.

Figure 6
Figure 6: Illustration of steps 4.54.8. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

  1. Plot genetic overlapping as well as genetic unique influences by clicking and dragging the mouse over cells R2–R6 and S2–S6 to highlight the data.
  2. Click on the Insert menu.
  3. Click on Charts > Stacked Column.
  4. Repeat steps 4.9–4.11 for shared environmental and non-shared environmental overlapping as well as unique influences. Choose cells T2–T6 and U2–U6 to plot shared environmental influences, and choose cells V2–V6 and W2–W6 for non-shared environmental influences.

Using Cholesky Decomposition to Explore Individual Differences in Longitudinal Relations between Reading Skills

Learning Objectives

Standardized estimates for genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental influences from the multivariate Cholesky decomposition model are depicted in Figure 7. In general, results revealed that individual differences in kindergarten pre-reading and first grade word-level reading skills accounted for a large proportion of the variance of genetic (40%) as well as shared environmental (39%) influences on seventh grade reading comprehension. In addition, results alluded to a degree of unique sources coming into play for each individual reading skill at each grade.

Figure 7
Figure 7: Full multivariate Cholesky decomposition model with standardized path estimates of genetic and environmental influences. Measured variables are depicted as rectangles, and a latent variable as an oval. LNF = kindergarten letter naming fluency, PSF = kindergarten phoneme segmentation fluency, WLRS = first grade word-level reading skills, RC = seventh grade reading comprehension. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

As indicated in Figure 8, it appears there was a large share of unique genetic influences (dark green) on letter naming fluency in kindergarten (36%), phoneme segmentation fluency in kindergarten (40%), and reading comprehension in seventh grade (30%). In contrast, word-level reading skills were to a lesser extent associated with unique genetic influences that arise in first grade (20%). Genetic influences on word-level reading skills were mostly overlapping (light green) with genetic influences on letter naming fluency and phoneme segmentation fluency (40%).

Figure 8
Figure 8: Percentage of unique and overlapping genetic influences on each reading skill. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

Focusing on the shared environmental influences (see Figure 9), the results implied that overlapping (light blue) shared environment influenced letter naming fluency and phoneme segmentation fluency in kindergarten (9%). Similarly, overlapping shared environmental effects were reflected in word-level reading skills in first grade (15%) and reading comprehension in seventh grade (39%) that were also shared with kindergarten reading skills. Unique shared environmental factors (dark blue) were found for first grade word-level reading skills (15%). These influences were independent of shared environmental influences in kindergarten.

Figure 9
Figure 9: Percentage of unique and overlapping shared environmental influences on each reading skill. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

For the non-shared environmental influences (see Figure 10), the results suggested very little overlap between factors (light yellow). Most non-shared environmental influences indicated unique influences (dark yellow) at each individual time point (i.e., grade).

Figure 10
Figure 10: Percentage of unique and overlapping non-shared environmental influences on each reading skill. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

The general representation of genetic and environmental factors underlying reading skills from elementary to middle school is shown in Figure 11. In general, it was shown that reading skills appear to be influenced by both genetic and environmental factors across this developmental period.

Figure 11
Figure 11: Total percentage of genetic, shared environmental, and non-shared environmental influences on each reading skill. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.

List of Materials

Microsoft Office Excel Microsoft
Microsoft Office Powerpoint Microsoft
Microsoft Office Visio Microsoft
Microsoft Office Word Microsoft
Mplus Statistical Program Mplus

Preparação do Laboratório

The Cholesky decomposition method is the gold standard used in the field of behavioral genetics. The method is popular because it is easy to program and solve. Using this method, researchers can explore individual differences in longitudinal relations of different variables across multiple time points. The method allows investigators to decompose variance into (1) unique genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that arise at specific time points as well as (2) overlapping genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that carry over from one time point to another. However, the method does not identify the mechanisms or origins underlying these effects. The current report focuses on application of the Cholesky decomposition method in the field of educational psychology. Specifically, it discusses individual differences in longitudinal relations between kindergarten letter knowledge, kindergarten phonological awareness, first grade word-level reading skills, and seventh grade reading comprehension.

The Cholesky decomposition method is the gold standard used in the field of behavioral genetics. The method is popular because it is easy to program and solve. Using this method, researchers can explore individual differences in longitudinal relations of different variables across multiple time points. The method allows investigators to decompose variance into (1) unique genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that arise at specific time points as well as (2) overlapping genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that carry over from one time point to another. However, the method does not identify the mechanisms or origins underlying these effects. The current report focuses on application of the Cholesky decomposition method in the field of educational psychology. Specifically, it discusses individual differences in longitudinal relations between kindergarten letter knowledge, kindergarten phonological awareness, first grade word-level reading skills, and seventh grade reading comprehension.

Procedimento

The Cholesky decomposition method is the gold standard used in the field of behavioral genetics. The method is popular because it is easy to program and solve. Using this method, researchers can explore individual differences in longitudinal relations of different variables across multiple time points. The method allows investigators to decompose variance into (1) unique genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that arise at specific time points as well as (2) overlapping genetic, shared and non-shared environmental effects that carry over from one time point to another. However, the method does not identify the mechanisms or origins underlying these effects. The current report focuses on application of the Cholesky decomposition method in the field of educational psychology. Specifically, it discusses individual differences in longitudinal relations between kindergarten letter knowledge, kindergarten phonological awareness, first grade word-level reading skills, and seventh grade reading comprehension.

Tags