5.4:

Principles of Classical Conditioning

JoVE Core
Introduction to Psychology
A subscription to JoVE is required to view this content.  Sign in or start your free trial.
JoVE Core Introduction to Psychology
Principles of Classical Conditioning

13 Views

01:23 min

October 24, 2024

Classical conditioning, as described by Ivan Pavlov, is a foundational concept in associative learning, where a neutral stimulus becomes capable of eliciting a conditioned response through association with an unconditioned stimulus. The process of acquisition, where this learning occurs, and the subsequent phenomena of contiguity, contingency, generalization, discrimination, extinction, and spontaneous recovery are crucial for a comprehensive understanding of classical conditioning.

During the acquisition phase, a neutral stimulus, such as a bell, is paired with an unconditioned stimulus (US), like meat powder. Initially, the neutral stimulus does not elicit any response. However, after repeated pairings where the bell precedes the meat powder, the neutral stimulus transforms into a conditioned stimulus (CS), eliciting a conditioned response (CR) of salivation. This learning process relied on two key principles: contiguity and contingency.

Contiguity refers to the temporal closeness between the CS and the US. For effective conditioning, the bell and the meat powder must be paired closely in time, typically within a fraction of a second. If there is a significant delay between the two stimuli, the association weakens. Contingency, on the other hand, involves the predictability of the US following the CS. The bell must consistently predict the arrival of food for a strong association to form. If the bell rings at random times without being followed by food, the dog will not reliably associate the bell with food, thereby weakening the conditioned response.

Generalization occurs when stimuli similar to the conditioned stimulus also trigger the conditioned response. For example, a dog conditioned to salivate to a bell may also salivate to a whistle, as it perceives the whistle as similar to the bell. Discrimination, conversely, involves the dog learning to respond only to the specific conditioned stimulus (the bell) and not to other similar stimuli. This differentiation occurs if the other stimuli, such as different sounds, are not paired with the unconditioned stimulus (food).

Extinction is the process by which the conditioned response diminishes and eventually disappears when the conditioned stimulus is repeatedly presented without the unconditioned stimulus. If the bell rings without being followed by food, the dog gradually stops salivating in response to the bell. However, spontaneous recovery can occur, where the extinguished conditioned response reappears after a period of rest. For instance, days after extinction, the dog might briefly salivate again upon hearing the bell, indicating that the learned association is not entirely erased but rather suppressed.

These principles of classical conditioning—acquisition, generalization, discrimination, extinction, and spontaneous recovery—demonstrate the dynamic nature of associative learning. Pavlov's research not only unveiled the mechanisms by which stimuli become associated but also provided a robust framework for understanding behavior modification and learning processes across various contexts.