概要

Extinction Training During the Reconsolidation Window Prevents Recovery of Fear

Published: August 24, 2012
doi:

概要

Conditioned fear can be diminished through an inhibitory process called extinction, but can resurface under conditions such as the passage of time or exposure to stress. Our protocol presents a novel way of preventing fear recovery by introducing extinction during the reconsolidation window (the re-storage phase of a reactivated memory).

Abstract

Fear is maladaptive when it persists long after circumstances have become safe. It is therefore crucial to develop an approach that persistently prevents the return of fear. Pavlovian fear-conditioning paradigms are commonly employed to create a controlled, novel fear association in the laboratory. After pairing an innocuous stimulus (conditioned stimulus, CS) with an aversive outcome (unconditioned stimulus, US) we can elicit a fear response (conditioned response, or CR) by presenting just the stimulus alone1,2 . Once fear is acquired, it can be diminished using extinction training, whereby the conditioned stimulus is repeatedly presented without the aversive outcome until fear is no longer expressed3. This inhibitory learning creates a new, safe representation for the CS, which competes for expression with the original fear memory4. Although extinction is effective at inhibiting fear, it is not permanent. Fear can spontaneously recover with the passage of time. Exposure to stress or returning to the context of initial learning can also cause fear to resurface3,4.

Our protocol addresses the transient nature of extinction by targeting the reconsolidation window to modify emotional memory in a more permanent manner. Ample evidence suggests that reactivating a consolidated memory returns it to a labile state, during which the memory is again susceptible to interference5-9. This window of opportunity appears to open shortly after reactivation and close approximately 6hrs later5,11,16, although this may vary depending on the strength and age of the memory15. By allowing new information to incorporate into the original memory trace, this memory may be updated as it reconsolidates10,11. Studies involving non-human animals have successfully blocked the expression of fear memory by introducing pharmacological manipulations within the reconsolidation window, however, most agents used are either toxic to humans or show equivocal effects when used in human studies12-14. Our protocol addresses these challenges by offering an effective, yet non-invasive, behavioral manipulation that is safe for humans.

By prompting fear memory retrieval prior to extinction, we essentially trigger the reconsolidation process, allowing new safety information (i.e., extinction) to be incorporated while the fear memory is still susceptible to interference. A recent study employing this behavioral manipulation in rats has successfully blocked fear memory using these temporal parameters11. Additional studies in humans have demonstrated that introducing new information after the retrieval of previously consolidated motor16, episodic17, or declarative18 memories leads to interference with the original memory trace14. We outline below a novel protocol used to block fear recovery in humans.

Protocol

概要

The study we describe in this protocol is conducted over three consecutive days, with a between-subject design of three experimental groups. On day 1, all subjects undergo fear conditioning, where they learn to discriminate between two cues, one that predicts a shock and another that doesn’t. On day 2, all subjects undergo extinction training, but differ regarding to what happens beforehand: Two groups receive a reminder cue to reactivate the fear memory before extinction training whereas a third group receives no such cue. One group receives the reminder cue 10 min before extinction, thus they undergo extinction while the fear memory is reconsolidating. The other group receives the reminder cue 6 hr before extinction, thus they undergo extinction after this window has presumably closed. The third group receives no reminder cue before undergoing extinction, thus they undergo standard extinction training. On Day 3, all subjects return for another extinction session. Their level of fear when they first see the conditioned cue on Day 3 indicates whether the fear memory remains. All three days are conducted in the same experimental setting, which is a sound-attenuated, temperature-controlled room. The conditioned stimuli are colored squares presented on a computer screen. We induce and measure fear with a mild electric shock and skin conductance, respectively, using a Biopac system module (see Table 1). A desktop computer is used to run the AcqKnowledge software program that allows for the recording and monitoring of SCR while a separate computer is used to run and present the experimental script to the subject. The subjects undergo all three stages around the same time of the day.

1. Fear Assessment with Skin Conductance Response (SCR) – Electrode Application & Recording

  1. The experiment room should ideally be kept warm (we set our rooms somewhere between 70 and 75 degrees) in order to facilitate reliable and measurable SCR. Clean the subject’s right or left fingertips using a q-tip dampened with distilled water; we do not recommend abrasive cleansors as they can dry the skin, disrupting sweat responses. Ensure fingertips are completely dry prior to electrode application.
  2. Apply highly conductive electrolyte gel to both electrodes (here we use “Signa Gel” by Parker Laboratories Inc., and electrodes connected to an MP100 Data Acquisition System made by Biopac Systems). Allow gel to rise slightly above the SCR electrode well and ensure there are no air bubbles within the gel as this can interefere with the SCR signal.
  3. Attach an electrode to the first and sec fingers of the hand between the first and sec phalanges, where the electrodermal signal is more pronounced. Ensure the electrode velcro straps are securely fastened and ask the subject to report whether they feel any pulsing in their fingertips. This pulsing indicates the straps have been applied too tightly, which can interefere with the SCR signal.
  4. Once skin conductance electrodes are secured, begin recording a skin conductance sample for approximately 5 min in order to test the subject’s electrodermal signal. Here, we use AcqKnowledge 3.9 software (from Biopac Systems Inc.) and record SCR waveforms on a MAC desktop computer. We use a sampling rate of 200Hz, a level appropriate for a signal as slow as SCR, however higher rates may be used. The recording screen should be separate from the computer presenting the experimental script and out of the subject’s sight. Ensure the subject has robust SCR responses by directing them to take in and hold a deep breath; this should produce a substantial rise in SCR (see Figure 1 for an example of such a response).

2. Fear Induction with Mild Electric Shock Stimulation – Electrode Application & Work-up Procedure

  1. On the wrist opposite the hand with the SCR electrodes, attach the stimulator bar. Here, we use a bar electrode (product code EL350 from Biopac Systems Inc.), connected to an SD9 Square Pulse Stimulator (made by Grass Technologies, an Atro-Med, Inc. Product Group) with a bead of conductance gel applied to each of the electrode heads.
  2. The shock trigger should be unplugged from the stimulator box during the work-up procedure. Place the bar on the inner wrist, on either side of the palmaris longus tendon and secure with nylon velcro strap in order to keep it in place during the experiment, but ensure the strap is not on too tight.
  3. Inform the subject that they will receive manually delivered shocks. Start at a low level (we recommend 20 v), and work up to a level that they determine is uncomfortable but not painful. We recommend the shock duration to be set at 200 ms, 50 pulses/sec, a standard level used in an array of past fear conditioning studies21,24, although others have used slightly different durations to induce the same sensation25,26.
  4. Manually trigger the shocks, starting at 20 volts. Continue triggering single shocks, gradually increasing the intensity in 5v increments, until the subject indicates that shocks are uncomfortable, but not painful. There should be a large SCR to the shocks (see Figure 2 for an example of a response to the shock). Record the final shock level and maintain this voltage level for the remainder of the experiment.

3. Day 1 – Acquisition

  1. Direct the subject to keep their eyes on the screen at all times, find a comfortable position that will not require movement and breathe naturally since abrupt respiratory changes can interfere with SCR. Both arms should be placed in a relaxed position with wrists up on the table/desk in front of subject or on the arms of a comfortable chair. Tell the subject that they will see visual stimuli on the screen and receive occasional shocks. Instruct the subject to monitor the relationship between the stimuli they are seeing and when a shock is received.
  2. Plug the trigger back into the Stimulator and change the trigger on AcqKnowledge setting from OFF to EXTERNAL. Once the subject is ready to begin, place the voltage back up to the recorded level, and initiate both the AcqKnowledge file and the experimental script. Here, we use E-prime Software version 2.0 from Psychology Software Tools, Inc.
  3. During the acquisition session, one conditioned stimulus (here, a yellow square) co-terminates with an aversive outcome (here, a mild shock) on a subset (38%) of trials; we refer to this as our CS+. The other conditioned stimulus (here, a blue square) is never paired with the US; we refer to this as our CS-. We use partial reinforcement to slow learning since previous work has found that 100% reinforcement results in rapid extinction once subjects realize the relationship between the CS+ and US has changed21. A partial reinforcement paradigm also allows us to limit our analysis to non-reinforced CS+ trials, thus excluding CS+ related SCRs that are potentially contaminated by physiological responses to shock (see Figure 3 for examples of conditioned and unconditioned SCRs to each CS type).
  4. Conditioned stimuli (CSs) are presented for 4 sec. There is a 10-12 sec random variable inter-trial-interval (ITI), during which subjects view a black screen with a white fixation cross in the center. Each session begins with a screen indicating the experiment is about to start (*READY*) and terminates with a screen indicating that they had successfully completed that part of the experiment (*YOU HAVE COMPLETED THIS PART OF THE EXPERIMENT*). During acquisition, a total number of 10 unreinforced trials CS+ trials and 10 CS- trials are presented in addition to 6 CS+ trials that are reinforced by shock.
  5. Produce two distinct pseudo-randomized orders for the whole experiment such that no trial-type (CS+, CS- or CS+US) repeats more than two times. Within each order, counterbalance the colors of the CSs. This should result in 4 orders whose assignment is randomized between individuals within each group. We recommend including at least one occurrence of consecutive reinforced CS+US trials to prevent subjects from assuming they will never receive consecutives shocks (see Table 2 for an example of trial order).

4. Day 2 – Reactivation & Extinction Training

Subject breakdown:

Randomly divide all subjects that have demonstrated adequate acquisition of fear from day 1 (see Exclusion Criteria below) into three groups:

Group 1 (reactivation-10 min): receives extinction training 10 minutes after retrieval within the reconsolidation window

Group 2 (reactivation-6 hr): receives extinction training 6 hours after retrieval outside of the reconsolidation window

Group 3 (no-reactivation): receives standard extinction training without any retrieval

  1. Upon arrival, each subject should be set up as per steps 1 – 2.2, with the same shock level set on Day 1 (no work-up is required). There are no new instructions. Tell the subject to sit comfortably and pay attention to the stimuli presented, just as the previous day.
  2. Subjects in the reactivation groups (both 10 min and 6 hr) undergo a single, unreinforced presentation of the CS+ (no shock) followed by a 10-min break. Subjects in the no-reactivation group undergo the set-up procedure, but then go directly to the break without the reactivation session. During the break, stop the AcqKnowledge recording of the SCR, turn the stimulator voltage to zero and direct subjects’ attention to a separate screen that has a pre-selected TV show (e.g., here, we used a randomly selected ~10 min portion of a “Simpsons” episode). Subjects will watch it for 10 min while all electrodes remain attached.
  3. After the break, the reactivation-10 min group proceeds with extinction training. Subjects in the reactivation-6 hr group return for extinction training 6 hr later. For the no-reactivation subjects, half immediately undergo extinction training, and half return 6 hr later for training. This group controls for the reminder cue, as well as different time lapses between reactivation and extinction training in each reminder group. Additionally, since subjects in the reactivation-6 hr group might undergo extinction either in the afternoon or evening (to allow ample time to elapse after reactivation) it is important to run subjects in the reactivation-10 min and no-reactivation group in the morning, afternoon and evening, to allow an even distribution of subjects at each time of the day.
  4. During extinction, a total number of 10 unreinforced CS+ trials and 11 CS- trials are presented to each reactivation group. Since the CS- is not presented during the reactivation session, an extra presentation should be added to the extinction session so all CSs are presented an equal number of times during Day 2. The no-reactivation group should receive 11 unreinforced presentation of each CS.
  5. In the extinction session, resume the AcqKnowledge recording file and return stimulator voltage to the set level. Instruct the subject to attend to the experimental screen where an extinction script is waiting on *READY* screen. A reminder of the previous day’s directions is often useful. Resume with the extinction training script, whereby the subject receives repeated presentations of both CSs without shock (see Table 2).

5. Day 3 – Spontaneous Fear Recovery Assay

  1. If the subject demonstrates adequate extinction learning (see Exclusion Criteria below) they should return to the lab 24 hr after their extinction session. Upon arrival, they should be set up as per steps 1 and 2.2; voltage is set to the same level as the acquisition session. Remind subjects of the directions from the previous days.
  2. Each subject would undergo a re-extinction session, whereby they again receive 10 unreinforced presentations of both CSs (see Table 2) in order to assess the extent to which fear responses recover due to the passage of time. The first trial on Day 3 should be an extra CS- (see Table 3) and is disregarded due to the orienting response at the beginning of the session. (The same can be done at the start of Day 2, however, since only the data from late acquisition is used to assess extinction learning (see Exclusion Criteria below) and in the final analysis, this recommendation is not critical for Day 2.) The next trial on Day 3 should be counterbalanced between CS+ and CS-. Spontaneous recovery is measured by subtracting the SCR during the first CS+ trial during re-extinction from the last CS+ trial during extinction.
  3. At the end of the session the subject is debriefed and paid $40 before departing.

6. Pre-processing & Scoring SCR Data

  1. Analyze the data offline using AcqKnowledge software. Here, we set three parameters to be measured from our data chart: t (time), delta-t (latency), and p-p (peak-peak). These measurements record the time-point, latency and peak amplitude of the selected SCR. Begin a new journal graph to record your measurements. Pass waveforms through a low-pass filter in order to reduce high frequency data by selecting Transform→Digital filter→FIR→ Low pass (we use the default settings provided in the Acknowledge Software Guide’s: cut off frequency = 25; 32 coefficients; filter entire wave). Smooth the data in order to reduce noise by selecting Transform→smoothing→ transform entire wave (we use the default settings provided in the Acknowledge Software Guide’s: mean value smoothing; smoothing factor = 3 samples; filter entire wave; however, with higher sampling rates or increased noise, the smoothing factor can be increased).
  2. Select a skin conductance response during each trial by taking the base to peak difference for the largest waveform (in microsiemens, μs) that begins within the 0.5 to 4.5 sec window following stimulus onset. The onset of the SCR must begin within this window to qualify as a stimulus-specific response, but may take longer to peak. SCRs may be recorded until they reach peak amplitude, however, responses that peak after 5 sec are not considered stimulus-specific and should be disregarded. This is precisely why reinforced trials should not be recorded: a SCR that is still peaking at the end of the trial may be tainted by the shock and produce subsequent increases in SCR that are not related to the CS itself.
  3. A response peak criterion of 0.02 μs should be utilized; responses below this criterion are typically regarded as noise and encoded as zero. After scoring, all unreinforced trials for each subject should be divided by that respective subject’s mean US response. This is derived by taking the average SCR for all shock trials across the acquisition session and accounts for individual differences in subject’s shock reactivity. Finally, all trials should be square root transformed to reduce skewness.

Timing

Day 1

Preparation: 5-7 min

Acquisition Script: ≈9 min

Total: 15 min

Day 2

Preparation: 5-7 min

Reactivation Script: ≈30 sec

Break: 10 min

Extinction Script: ≈5.5 min

Total: 20 min

Day 3

Preparation: 5-7 min

Re-Extinction Script: ≈6.5 min

Total: 15 min

7. Representative Results for Each Group

At the end of day 1 (acquisition), subjects should demonstrate differential SCR to the CSs, that is, higher responding to the CS+ as compare to the CS- (see Figure 4, acquisition, for an example from individual subjects). By the end of day 2 (reactivation/extinction), this differential response should diminish, such that there is no longer a difference between CS+ and CS- (see Figure 4, extinction, for an example from individual subjects). When examining spontaneous recovery on Day 3, we would expect to see a lack of fear recovery in the reactivation-10 min group since this group received extinction training within the reconsolidation window, thus allowing the original memory trace to reconsolidate with this new safety information, and preventing the recovery of fear responses (see Figure 4, top panel, for an example from an individual subject). We would expect to see substantial recovery of fear, however, for the CS+ (not CS-) in the reactivation-6 hr group that received extinction training outside of the reconsolidation window (see Figure 4, middle panel, for an example from an individual subject) as well as the no-reactivation group that received standard extinction training (see Figure 4, bottom panel, for an example from an individual subject).

Figure 1
Figure 1. Example of SCR during electrode set up. Black, vertical arrows indicate the point at which subject was instructed to take a deep breath which resulted in a marked increase in SCR. Click here to view larger figure.

Figure 2
Figure 2. Example of SCR during shock work-up procedure. Black vertical arrows indicate point at which shock was administered. Grey arrows indicate the peak of skin conductance rise in response to the shock. Click here to view larger figure.

Figure 3
Figure 3. Example of conditioned response (CR) to the onset of the CS+ followed by a larger unconditioned response (UR) to the delivery of shock (US); subsequent trial is that of a CS-. Grey arrows indicate the CS onset and offset; black arrows indicate the delivery of US. Click here to view larger figure.

Figure 4
Figure 4. Example of acquisition, extinction and spontaneous recovery in an individual subject from each experimental condition (top panel: reactivation-10 min; middle panel: reactivation-6 hr; bottom panel: no reactivation).

Table 1
Table 1. Table of relevant products and equipment.

Table 2
Table 2. Reconsolidation protocol and timeline.

Table 3
Table 3. An example of trial order for each phase of the experiment. Note one extra CS- should be added to Day 2.2 (extinction) in both reminder groups in order to account for the reactivation of the CS+. The first CS- trial on Day 3 (denoted with grey background) is disregarded due to the orienting response at the beginning of the session.

Experimental Set up

The setup includes a PC running E-prime, a Mac running acknowledge, an MP100 Biopac system, and a shock box. In-line code is programmed into the E-prime script to communicate with the Acknowledge software and the shock box. This enables E-prime to send signals to Acknowledge denoting event onsets, and signals to the shock box to trigger shock administration. The output from E-prime is sent via a printer port to the Biopac system, which in turn sends the event signal to the Mac recording physiology with Acknowledge via a USB connection provided with the Biopac system, as well as to the shock box (see Table 1 for product information).

Exclusion Criteria

Subjects should be excluded before completing all experimental sessions or from data analysis if they demonstrate any of the following:

  • Failure to show detectable or reliable SCR – when applying and testing skin conductance response, subjects may demonstrate flat SCR. If this occurs, it is recommended to wait an additional 5 min as responses may not immediately emerge, or check for bubbles with in the conductance gel (in this case electrodes should be removed, cleaned and refilled). Subjects who have flat SCR response even after these troubleshooting strategies should be excused from the experiment.
  • Failure to demonstrate acquisition of the fear response (on Day 1): differential SCR to the CS+ and CS- by the end of the acquisition session (mean of second half of the acquisition trials) that is in the wrong direction (i.e. CS- > CS+) or that is too small to conclude learning has occurred (i.e., difference is less than 0.1 μs).
  • Failure to demonstrate extinction of the fear response (on Day 2): differential SCR to the CS+ and CS- by the end of the extinction session (on the last trial) that is larger than 0.1 μs.

Discussion

The protocol we presented here enables creating a simple conditioned fear response to a neutral stimulus paired with an aversive outcome. A day later we reactivated the fear memory in order to trigger its reconsolidation. During this stage we induced interference in the form of extinction training. To verify that extinction affects reconsolidation we used two control groups. One group underwent extinction without the reminder trial triggering reconsolidation, and another group experienced the reminder trial but then underwent extinction outside the reconsolidation window. We then measured, during the next day, whether the memory spontaneously recovered or whether the fear response to the conditioned stimuli failed to recover.

Pavlovian conditioning is a classic paradigm that has been widely used in the last few decades across many species. Animal research has described the neural mechanisms underlying the formation of the fear association and its extinction in great detail19,20. Studies in humans have successfully adapted this protocol to elucidate the neural correlates in the human brain21-24. The current protocol is a subtle variation of this well-established paradigm and therefore ideal to reveal, characterize and manipulate reconsolidation in humans.

開示

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Acknowledgements

We thank D. Johnson and K. Doelling for assistance with data collection. We also thank M.-H. Monfils, J. LeDoux, Y. Niv and M. Milad for advice on the experimental protocols. This study was funded by the James S. McDonnell Foundation and National Institutes of Health (NIH) grant R21 MH072279 (E.A.P.), NIH grants R37 MH038774, P50 MH058911, RO1 MH046516 and K05 MH067048 (J.E.L.), Postdoctoral fellowships NSERC, CIHR and AHFMR (M.-H.M.), and a Fulbright and Blavatnik awards (D.S.).

参考文献

  1. Pavlov, I. P., Anrep, G. V. . Reflexes. , (1927).
  2. LeDoux, J. E. Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 23, 155-184 (2000).
  3. Myers, K. M., Davis, M. Behavioral and neural analysis of extinction. Neuron. 36, 567-584 (2002).
  4. Bouton, M. E. Context, ambiguity, and unlearning : sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry. 52, 976-986 (2002).
  5. Nader, K., Schafe, G. E., LeDoux, J. E. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval. Nature. 406, 722-726 (2000).
  6. Nader, K. Memory traces unbound. Trends in Neuroscience. 26, 65-72 (2003).
  7. Alberini, C. M. Mechanisms of memory stabilization: are consolidation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes. Trends in Neuroscience. 28, 51-56 (2005).
  8. Sara, S. J., Hars, B. In memory of consolidation. Learning and Memory. 13, 515-521 (2006).
  9. Dudai, Y. Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused. Current Opinions in Neurobiology. 16, 174-178 (2006).
  10. Schiller, D., Monfils, M. -. H., Raio, C. M., Johnson, D. C., LeDoux, J. E., Phelps, E. A. . Nature. 463, 49-54 (2010).
  11. Monfils, M. -. H., Cowansage, K. K., Klann, E., LeDoux, J. E. Extinction-reconsolidation boundaries: Key to persistent attenuation of fear memories. Science. 324, 951-955 (2009).
  12. Brunet, A. Effect of post- retrieval propranololon psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research. 42, 503-506 (2008).
  13. Kindt, M., Soeter, M., Vervliet, B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Nature Neuroscience. 12, 256-258 (2009).
  14. Schiller, D., Phelps, E. A. Does reconsolidation occur in humans. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 5, 24 (2011).
  15. Duvarci, S., Nader, K. Characterization of fear memory reconsolidation. Journal of Neuroscience. 24, 9269-9275 (2004).
  16. Walker, M. P., Brakefield, T., Hobson, J. A., Stickgold, R. Dissociable stages of human memory consolidation and reconsolidation. Nature. 425, 616-620 (2003).
  17. Hupbach, A., Gomez, R., Hardt, O., Nadel, L. Reconsolidation of episodic memories: A subtle reminder triggers integration of new information. Learning & Memory. 14, 1-7 (2007).
  18. Forcato, . Reconsolidation of declarative memory in humans. Learning & Memory. 14, 295-303 (2007).
  19. McNally, G. P., Johanses, J. P., Blair, H. T. Placing prediction into the fear circuit. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience. 34, 283-292 (2011).
  20. Herry, C., Ferraguti, F., Singewald, N., Letzkus, J. J., Ehrlich, I., Luthi, A. Neural circuits of fear extinction. European Journal of Neuroscience. 31, 599-612 (2010).
  21. Phelps, E. A., Delgado, M. R., Nearing, K. I., LeDoux, J. E. Extinction learning in humans: Role of the amygdala and vmPFC. Neuron. 43, 897-905 (2004).
  22. Milad, M. R., Quirk, G. J. Neurons in the medial prefrontal cortex signal memory for fear extinction. Nature. 420, 70-74 (2002).
  23. Schiller, D., Levy, I., Niv, Y., LeDoux, J. E., Phelps, E. A. From Fear to Safety and Back: Reversal of Fear in the Human Brain. Journal of Neuroscience. 28, 11517-11525 (2008).
  24. Schiller, D., Delgado, M. R. Overlapping neural systems mediating extinction, reversal and regulation of fear. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience. 14, 268-2627 (2010).
  25. Esteves, F., Parra, C., Dimberg, U., Ohman, A. Nonconscious associative learning: Pavlovian conditioning of skin conductance responses to masked fear-relevant facial stimuli. Psychophysiology. 31, 375-385 (1994).
  26. Knight, D. C., Cheng, D. T., Smith, C. N., Stein, E. A., Helmstetter, F. J. Neural Substrates Mediating Human Delay and Trace Fear Conditioning. The Journal of Neuroscience. 24, 218-228 (2004).
  27. Venables, P. H., Christie, M. J., Martin, I., Venables, P. H. Techniques in psychophysiology. Electrodermal activity. , 3-67 (1980).

Play Video

記事を引用
Schiller, D., Raio, C. M., Phelps, E. A. Extinction Training During the Reconsolidation Window Prevents Recovery of Fear. J. Vis. Exp. (66), e3893, doi:10.3791/3893 (2012).

View Video