Trace eyeblink classical conditioning (ECC) was used to assess hippocampal-dependent associative learning in adult rats that were administered a high concentration (11.9% v/v) of alcohol during early neonatal brain development. In general, ECC procedures are sound diagnostic tools for detecting brain dysfunction across many psychological and biomedical settings.
Neonatal rats were administered a relatively high concentration of ethyl alcohol (11.9% v/v) during postnatal days 4-9, a time when the fetal brain undergoes rapid organizational change and is similar to accelerated brain changes that occur during the third trimester in humans. This model of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASDs) produces severe brain damage, mimicking the amount and pattern of binge-drinking that occurs in some pregnant alcoholic mothers. We describe the use of trace eyeblink classical conditioning (ECC), a higher-order variant of associative learning, to assess long-term hippocampal dysfunction that is typically seen in alcohol-exposed adult offspring. At 90 days of age, rodents were surgically prepared with recording and stimulating electrodes, which measured electromyographic (EMG) blink activity from the left eyelid muscle and delivered mild shock posterior to the left eye, respectively. After a 5 day recovery period, they underwent 6 sessions of trace ECC to determine associative learning differences between alcohol-exposed and control rats. Trace ECC is one of many possible ECC procedures that can be easily modified using the same equipment and software, so that different neural systems can be assessed. ECC procedures in general, can be used as diagnostic tools for detecting neural pathology in different brain systems and different conditions that insult the brain.
It is quite hard to imagine that in today's day and age with better health care and access to health services, alcohol abuse remains a major global health concern. Unfortunately, it has been shown that an expectant mother who drinks a high amount of alcohol can have a child with severe brain damage and neurodevelopmental disorders that last a lifetime, as evident in those afflicted with fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS)1,2,3. In women with some confirmed history of maternal alcohol use, the developing fetus is also susceptible to small amounts of alcohol or different patterns of alcohol consumption that produce varying differences in blood alcohol concentrations. In this latter case, while the children may not exhibit the severe morphological or neurobehavioral disruptions as those with FAS, they may still exhibit lifelong cognitive disabilities and emotional disturbances that range from mild to severe3,4. Altogether, FAS and less severe forms of prenatal alcohol-mediated disruptions constitute a collection of fetal alcohol spectrum disorders (FASDs). It is no surprise that FASDs are completely preventable, but astonishingly estimates show that in populations where alcohol abuse is quite common, they remain the primary non-genetic cause of neural and cognitive disability, affecting about 2% to 5% of young US children and those in European countries such as France and Sweden. With respect to the incidence of FAS alone within the US, the prevalence is 2 to 7 per 1,000 live births5, implying that the overall incidence of FASDs to be much higher than that for FAS.
Neuroimaging studies conducted in children with FASDs have shown that they display brain abnormalities, such as a thinner corpus callosum6, smaller anterior cerebellar vermis7, and smaller hippocampus8. These brain abnormalities underlie some of the long-term neurocognitive disruptions observed in children with FASDs. The exact links that tie variations in maternal alcohol-mediated brain changes and variations in the profile (i.e., type, extent) of particular neurocognitive impairments have yet to be clearly determined. But as a starting point, the hippocampus is an excellent candidate for determining its susceptibility to prenatal alcohol effects. Indeed, children with FASDs exhibit deficits in hippocampal-mediated behaviors such as place learning9,10 and delayed object recall11.
Rodent models of FASDs have proven to be invaluable in elucidating the mechanisms leading to neurocognitive disruptions seen in children with FASDs. A well-established binge-exposure model that we have adopted involves delivering alcohol to rats during postnatal days 4-912,13, a period when the brain undergoes rapid synapse and dendritic contact formation, comparable to human fetal week 24 and extending into the 3rd trimester14,15,16,17. This particular model induces significant loss of hippocampal neurons18,19 and neurons in many other brain regions such as the cerebellum12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20,21,22,23, accompanied by severe impairments in cognitive functions spanning different domains21,24,25. Cognitive disruption from early alcohol exposure in rats may be assessed in different ways, particularly with eyeblink classical conditioning (ECC). ECC is a paradigm that has been utilized for more than a century to scientifically investigate the fundamental basis of learning26,27 and as such, provides a useful method to better understanding the adverse neurocognitive consequences resulting from fetal alcohol exposure. It is a very flexible paradigm that allows investigators to use a variety of different ECC procedures, any of which can be examined across many mammalian species ascending the phylogenetic scale (from mice to humans) and over different courses of brain development28,29,30,31. Furthermore, the fundamental neural circuits that mediate associative learning in this paradigm are supported by experimental and neuropsychological reports in these same species26,32,33,34,35,36,37.
One form of ECC, trace ECC is demonstrated in this paper (Figure 1). To provide context, it is compared against the more traditional form – delay ECC. The ECC paradigm was modeled after classical conditioning using dogs, first carried out by the Nobel-Prize winning physiologist, Ivan Pavlov. Pavlov discovered that certain stimuli such as tones do not naturally elicit salivation, but when it precedes and overlaps with the delivery of food, the salivary response can be strengthened from repeated presentations of the two, provided that this tone-food contingency is maintained. This is an example of delay ECC, with the notion that associative strength is mediated by immediate temporal contiguity between the two stimuli, thus making learning conditions optimal for an animal. He also tested other variations of the tone-food contingency, such as turning the tone off and leaving a "trace" period before delivering the food. When these two stimuli were discontiguous enough, it became much harder for the dogs to emit salivation responses prior to the delivery of the food. The discontiguity between the tone being turned off and the delivery of the food is thus an example of trace ECC. As rodents do not naturally salivate to the presence of food, more species-relevant stimuli such as mild shock are used instead; they also do not naturally emit defensive eyeblink responses to tones. With this backdrop, rodent ECC procedures involve presenting a tone at a given decibel level and pairing it in some fashion with mild shock to either the eyelid muscle (orbicularis oculi) or the temporalis muscle to elicit an eyeblink response. The tone is considered a conditioned stimulus (CS) while the shock is considered an unconditioned stimulus (US). In delay ECC, the CS is presented first; this stimulus remains on for a given duration. Afterwards, the US is delivered. These two stimuli overlap for a given duration, and then both terminate simultaneously; the resultant eyeblink response emitted due to the US is considered an unconditioned response (UR). In this procedure, rodents learn to emit eyeblink responses sometime after the CS is presented, but just before the US, in order to anticipate this aversive stimulus. The learned eyeblink response is referred to as a conditioned response (CR). For trace ECC, the CS and US are separated by a period of time that is void of stimuli known as a trace interval; they do not overlap in time as in delay ECC. During this interval, the animal is tasked to resolve the associational requirements between stimuli. Similar to delay ECC, learning occurs when the animal consistently emits a blink response after the CS turns off, but immediately before delivery of the US. Over some amount of acquisition training (CS paired with US), learning curves (i.e., based on different CR measurements) develop. Lesion and neuroimaging studies show that successful learning in delay ECC is dependent on having intact cerebellar-brain stem neuro-circuitry38,39,40, whereas trace ECC is a higher-order procedure that requires additional neural engagement from the hippocampus41,42,43,44 and other cortical structures45,46. Because of the timing-related requirements needed in order to acquire trace CRs successfully, this task is also more difficult to learn (even for normal subjects).
Figure 1: Trace eyeblink classical conditioning. An actual waveform is shown that is representative of an adult rat in the unintubated-control (UC) group. The tone CS (85 dB, 2.8 kHz) is first presented for 380 ms. A trace interval of 500 ms ensues, where no stimuli are present. Afterwards a shock US (1.6 mA) is delivered for 100 ms. Successful learning in this task occurs when the frequency (%) or amplitude (in volts) of eyeblinks during the conditioned response (CR) time window (Total CR period) increases over many sessions of training. In particular, rodents with an intact hippocampus will usually emit more well-timed CRs (Adaptive CRs) just prior to the onset of the shock US (within a 200-ms window). Startle responses (SRs) during the first 80 ms after tone CS onset and unconditioned responses (URs) are also measured. Non-associative SRs are typically low or nonexistent in well-trained rodents, while URs are expected to be high in frequency and amplitude. This task requires that the rodent learn to bridge the association between the CS offset and US onset (during the trace interval), therefore making it inherently more difficult to acquire compared to delay ECC. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Here we demonstrate the adverse functional consequences of neonatal alcohol exposure that is delivered in a binge-like manner, as assessed by a trace ECC procedure that delivers an 85 dB tone CS (2.8 kHz) which remains on for 380 ms, followed by a 1.6 mA shock US which remains on for 100 ms, and these stimuli are separated by a trace period of 500 ms. We have reported on the utility of this behavioral assay in previous studies examining choline intervention and iron supplementation in mitigating the effects of neonatal alcohol exposure18,47. Indeed, trace ECC can be used as a diagnostic tool to assess neonatal alcohol-induced hippocampal pathology. The advantage it has over delay ECC is that it is more sensitive to detecting disturbances in hippocampal function, which is compromised in humans with FASDs.
Demonstration of ECC extends far outside the fetal alcohol field. Many variants of ECC (e.g., delay, trace, compound, reversal) can be used to elucidate ontogenetic differences in learning across development, the neurobiological basis of associative learning in normal mammals, as well as the vulnerabilities of different brain systems to many challenges, including (but not limited to) teratogens, environmental toxins, traumatic brain injury, neurodegenerative diseases, and psychiatric conditions.
NOTE: All procedures were approved and carried out in accordance with the policies set forth by the East Carolina University IACUC. Long-Evans rats were generated from females mated with male breeders. Pups from all three treatment groups (see 1.1) were generated within a litter from the same dam. Five litters were produced and each litter was culled to 8 pups on postnatal day (PD) 3. The remaining two pups from each litter were assigned to separate experiments. Both male and female offspring (one per exposure group) were included in the study. A total of N = 27 adult offspring were examined in this study; 3 rats were excluded due to broken 3T wire leads (see 3.1.1) which were irreparable on Day 1 of ECC training.
1. Preparation of Groups, Materials, and Solutions
2. Neonatal Alcohol Exposure (Postnatal Days 4-9)
3. Fabrication and Modification of Electrodes
Figure 2: Electromyographic (EMG) recording electrodes and bipolar electrode. The finished EMG headstage (right, orange) is constructed from three male contact pins, two size 3T PTFE-coated wires, one size 10T PTFE-coated wire, and a modified micro strip. The three wires are approximately 5 cm each and are crimped to the contact pins. The finished bipolar electrode (right, white) is untwisted, re-straightened, and molded in a V-shape (5 mm split). Shielding is removed from the tips of the two prongs. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
4. Eyelid Surgery Procedure (Postnatal Day 90)
5. Trace Eyeblink Classical Conditioning Procedure
Figure 3: Modified operant conditioning box for eyeblink conditioning. Rats are freely-moving mammals, and therefore a rotating commutator is used for maintaining electrical signal contact from the EMG and bipolar plugs that are attached to the head. The commutator is attached to the arm of the stanchion, which is counter-weighted for alleviating pressure on the rat. A piezo tweeter (speaker) delivers a 2.8 kHz tone at 85 dB and these values are calibrated regularly. Acoustical foam assists with attenuating environmental noise. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4: Eyeblink conditioning system. This custom-built system consists of an EMG Integrator unit that filters and amplifies incoming signals from the rats, a Stimulus Control unit that delivers various stimuli in addition to tones and shocks, a pre-amplifier for each operant box to increase EMG signal gain, and a stimulus isolator for each operant box; it provides varying shock levels (in mA). A digital oscilloscope (not part of the stock eyeblink system) is used for diagnostic purposes during habituation and acquisition. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
The eyeblink software is capable of providing a large and comprehensive set of data for many types of measurements. For brevity, we report in this study, representative results for learning and performance measures that include adaptive CR percentage, adaptive CR amplitude, UR percentage and UR amplitude. The adaptive CR period was chosen as it represents the acquisition of well-timed eyeblink responses over repeated training, as a result of enhanced synaptic plasticity in the hippocampus during trace ECC50,51,52. The UR measures were chosen to elucidate whether the neonatal alcohol-induced learning deficits in trace ECC were due to disruptions in associative learning or disruptions in responding to the shock US – which may indicate motivational or motor differences, rather than learning differences among treatment groups. Data for each measure were analyzed using 2 (Sex) x 3 (Neonatal Group) x 6 (Session) mixed ANOVAs, with Session as the repeated-measures factor. Significant main effects for neonatal treatment were analyzed using Tukey's post hoc tests and significant interactions were analyzed using simple effects tests. All statistical analyses were conducted using a minimum alpha level of 0.05 and results in graphs are mean ± SEM.
Beginning with the adaptive CR percentage measure, the ANOVA indicated a significant main effect of neonatal group, F(2,21) = 11.69, p <0.001, but no significant main effect of sex (p = 0.71) or significant interaction between these factors (p = 0.20). As expected, adaptive CR percentage increased over the six sessions of training, F(5, 105) = 81.15, p <0.001, and the differences among neonatal groups were dependent on some level of session, F(10, 105) = 4.58, p <0.001. There were no other significant interactions involving the session factor. Likewise for adaptive CR amplitude, there was again a significant main effect of neonatal group, F(2,21) = 22.32, p <0.001, but no significant main effect of sex (p = 0.21) or significant interaction between these factors (p = 0.48). CR amplitude also increased significantly over the six sessions of training, F(5, 105) = 59.27, p <0.001, and the differences among neonatal groups were dependent on some level of session, F(10, 105) = 4.31, p <0.001. Overall, both CR measures showed significant differences among the group means and these means separated significantly at different sessions of training. To confirm which groups differed significantly, Tukey's post hoc tests showed that the alcohol-intubated (AI) rats performed significantly worse on both CR measures than the unintubated-control (UC) and sham-intubated (SI) rats (p <0.01 for CR percentage; p <0.001 for CR amplitude), which did not differ from each other (p's >0.05). Simple effects tests performed on the significant Neonatal Group x Session interactions for both CR measures, confirmed that the AI rats were more significantly impaired in acquiring CRs beginning at Session 2 and carrying on through Session 6 compared to both UC and SI rats (all p's <0.05), which did not differ from each other throughout six sessions. The only exception was adaptive CR amplitude for SI rats did not begin to differ significantly from AI rats until Session 3. These results are shown in Figure 5A, 5B.
There were no significant differences in the UR measures due to sex, neonatal group, or interactions of these factors with the session factor. These negative findings indicated that each group was able to emit eyeblink responses to the shock US equally, and that the learning deficits observed in the AI rats were not influenced by motivational or motor differences in blinking (Figure 6A, 6B).
Figure 5: Acquisition of trace conditioned responding (mean ± SEM). Early alcohol exposure (group AI) significantly affected acquisition of adaptive conditioned response (CR) percentage (A) and amplitude (B). Trace ECC is inherently difficult to acquire, therefore the measures are relatively lower for all groups – with delay ECC, percentages may reach 80-85% in rodent models of FASD21,53. Nonetheless, the trace ECC procedure is more taxing on the hippocampus, which is susceptible to alcohol effects during early brain development. * = p <0.05, ** = p <0.01, *** = p <0.001 between UC and AI rats; sample sizes are provided in parentheses. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 6: Acquisition of unconditioned responses (mean ± SEM). Eyeblink performance (UR percentage and UR amplitude) was not significantly different among groups. The lack of differences indicate that the shock intensity used during acquisition training did not differentially alter motivation in the AI rats or their ability to produce defensive blink responses to the shock, compared to both control groups (UC and SI). Sample sizes are provided in parentheses. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Neonatal rat pups that received ethyl alcohol during postnatal days 4-9 exhibited trace eyeblink conditioning impairments in adulthood. These findings support the idea that alcohol is a teratogen with enduring detrimental effects on hippocampal function. Overall, conditioned responding in the trace procedure was lower for rats exposed to alcohol compared to rats in both control groups. The associative learning impairments in the alcohol-exposed rats were not influenced by motivational or motor differences (i.e., no differences in blinking to shock US intensity).
While trace ECC is a useful diagnostic tool for elucidating challenge-induced hippocampal neuropathology, the results from this method must be placed into proper context. First, the key procedural elements in this demonstration involved the targeted delivery of alcohol during a known window of vulnerability to the developing brain, fabrication of electrode hardware that allows recording of electromyographic activity and delivers shock, surgical implantation of the aforementioned hardware, and subsequent animal testing using a learning paradigm that assesses a cognitive function of interest. At each stage in the process, care must be taken to not cause unnecessary/unintended harm to the rodent subjects and to monitor their health signs regularly. Their behavioral results provide the "window" to cognition, a psychological construct that is only accurately described when their health is not compromised by experimental errors encompassing alcohol dosing, hardware defects, or surgical implantation. Thus, each procedural element in the research process must be implemented in a sound manner in order to ensure that results from ECC can be extrapolated to findings in humans. Secondly, the ECC paradigm provides insight on the nature of associative learning, but care must be taken not to extend findings using this approach and broadly ascribe them to other cognitive domains – such as working memory, short/long-term memories, and consciousness – unless one has incorporated some facet of these domains within an ECC study by experimental design. For example, this demonstration examined the acquisition phase of trace ECC learning, but did not examine memory retention in the rats after they completed the training. Memory is thus an independent psychological process that should be evaluated in addition to learning. By design, one may incorporate a memory retention interval to assess either short-term or long-term memory ability. Thirdly, recognition that there are parallel memory systems54 that may work simultaneously along with motivational, experiential, and hormonal factors which contribute to behavior, is essential for understanding that associativity (during ECC) is but one of many processes that reveal what is "good" or "poor" about learning. Lastly, trace ECC is not a purely hippocampal-dependent task, as other brain regions may mediate some component of the CR. Thus, an understanding of the interactions between different neural circuits and/or the type of stimulus parameters that are utilized in a study, must be taken into account when making implications based on discrete results. The cerebellum, for example, also contributes to trace ECC, where it influences the topographical characteristics of the CR and CR timing, particularly when the ISI is short in duration. Trace ECC is not affected in humans with cerebellar damage who are tested with a long trace interval (1,000 ms), but is affected in those who receive a shorter trace interval (400 ms)34. Furthermore, bilateral lesions of the dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) that target the anterior cingulate and medial agranular regions in mice, prevent acquisition of trace CRs55, while destruction of the caudal mPFC in rabbits produces similar results46. These findings also highlight the importance of considering species differences in prefrontal contributions to cerebellar-brain stem driven associative learning, such as trace ECC. While neonatal alcohol exposure during PD 4-9 adversely affected acquisition of 500-ms trace CRs for adult rats in this study and others47,56, this is not the same case for neonatal alcohol-exposed rats that experience a 300-ms trace interval, even when challenged at a relatively high dose of alcohol (5 g/kg)57, suggesting that the trace impairment in alcohol-exposed rats is dependent on the duration of the trace interval.
In this study, the hippocampus was emphasized as being vitally important for mediating trace ECC, and when challenged by neonatal alcohol exposure, exhibits neural-related damage as reflected by impairments in acquisition of trace CRs. It must cautioned, however, that the cerebellum-brain stem circuitry, particularly the interpositus nucleus, is essential for many facets of ECC, including the acquisition, expression, and topographical features of the CR, depending on the type of ECC task including trace ECC36,40,55,58,59. Indeed, this neural circuit interacts with the hippocampus for driving the expression of CRs during higher-order forms of ECC, such as trace ECC60. Whether alcohol exposure during early brain development specifically affects hippocampal function in trace ECC is not entirely clear. Many different brain regions are vulnerable to early alcohol insult, including the mPFC, cerebellum, and hippocampus18,19,23,47,61,62, and it is very likely that alcohol disrupts the functioning of these structures to varying degrees and to varying, but functionally important differences across many ECC procedures. In spite of the pitfalls regarding the interpretation of results from trace ECC studies, successful acquisition of trace CRs has been shown to at least rely on an intact hippocampus, as supported by animal lesion studies42,44,63,64,65. This procedure thus remains a highly valuable approach for demonstrating the links between developmental alcohol exposure to trace conditioned responding because the neural circuitry underlying it, is much better understood than that of other hippocampal-dependent tasks, such as place learning in the Morris water maze, novel object recognition, and contextual and trace fear conditioning.
ECC as a behavioral method to "assay" cognition, has widespread applicability in the field of developmental neuroteratology. Indeed, recent findings from our lab support the notion that the developing hippocampus is highly sensitive to alcohol effects, which may be mitigated by different interventional strategies18,47. The key benefit here is that with a better understanding of alcohol-induced trace ECC learning deficits, they may be predictive of other problems in hippocampal-based functions outside of associative learning – particularly those known to be mediated by the same hippocampal neurocircuitry.
Application of trace ECC and its other variants (e.g., delay, reversal, discrimination, compound) to elucidate the neurobiological mechanisms and neural systems involved in associative learning, can be extended beyond the field of fetal alcohol research. For example, this paradigm has received much attention in human cases and animal models of psychiatric conditions such as schizophrenia66,67, neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer's disease68,69, and drugs of abuse70,71,72. Its benefits as a research method to assess neurocognitive function and dysfunction are thus evident across many psychological and biomedical disciplines, including neuroscience.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
This work was supported by a grant to TDT from the Alcohol Beverage Medical Research Foundation (ABMRF).
Neonatal Alcohol Exposure | |||
190 Proof Ethyl Alcohol (USP) | Pharmco-AAPER | 225-36000 [ECU Medical Storeroom] | Can be substituted; should be USP; avoid using 200 proof ethyl alcohol |
Container/Basket for Pups | Any | ||
Corn Oil | Any | Food grade | |
Heated Water Therapy Pump w/ Pads | Gaymar | TP-500 | To keep pups warm; can be substituted |
Hypodermic Needles 22G x 1 in, Sterile | Any | ||
Hypodermic Needles 30G x 1/2 in, Sterile | Any | ||
Isopropyl Alcohol 70% | EMD Millipore | PX1840-4 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted; reagent grade www.fishersci.com |
Long-Evans Rats (Female and Male Breeders) | Charles River Laboratories | N/A [ECU Dept. of Comparative Medicine] | Age and weight need to be specified; pricing varies by these factors www.criver.com |
Micro Dissecting Scissors, 3.5 in, 23 mm Blades | Biomedical Research Instruments | 11-2200 | For cutting PE tubing brisurgical.com |
Polyethylene 10 Tubing (0.011 in. I.D.; 0.024 in. O.D.) | BD Diagnostic Systems | 22-204008 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Polyethylene 50 Tubing (0.023 in. I.D.; 0.038 in. O.D.) | BD Diagnostic Systems | 22270835 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Regulated water heater or baby milk bottle warmer | Any | Optional; helps with warming up cold milk solutions | |
Tuberculin Syringes, Sterile, 1.0 ml | Any | ||
Tuberculin Syringes, Sterile, 10 ml | Any | Can be used to draw out ethyl alcohol or use appropriate size micropipet | |
Weigh Scale | Any | Should have good resolution (in gram units) | |
Name | Company | Catalog Number | Comments |
EMG Headstage Fabrication and Bipolar Electrode Modification | |||
Bipolar Electrode, 2 Channel SS Twisted | Plastics One, Inc. | MS303/2-B/SPC ELECT SS 2C TW .008" | Must specify custom length of 20 mm below pedestal www.plastics1.com |
Centi-Loc Strip Socket Insulator (aka, Micro Strip) | ITT Cannon / ITT Interconnect Solutions | CTA4-IS-60* or CTA4-1S-60* | *Depends on vendor; see www.onlinecomponents.com or www.avnetexpress.avnet.com |
Dental Pliers, Serrated | CMF Medicon | 390.20.05 | Can be substituted; use to crimp wires to male contact pins www.medicon.de |
Micro Dissecting Scissors, 3.5 in, 23 mm Blades | Biomedical Research Instruments | 11-2200 | Only use to cut 3T wires; cutting 10T wires will damage the blade – use the blade of the wire stripper instead brisurgical.com |
PTFE-Coated Stainless Steel Wire, 10T (Bare Diameter .010 in) | Sigmund Cohn-Medwire | 316SS10T | www.sigmundcohn.com |
PTFE-Coated Stainless Steel Wire, 3T (Bare Diameter 0.003 in) | Sigmund Cohn-Medwire | 316SS3T | www.sigmundcohn.com |
Razor Blade | Any | To strip 1 mm from prongs of bipolar electrode | |
Relia-Tac Socket Contact Pin, Male | Cooper Interconnect | 220-P02-100 | See Allied Electronics Cat # 70144761 www.alliedelec.com |
Tweezers, High Precision, Serrated, 4 3/4 in | Electron Microscopy Sciences | 78314-00D | To grasp 10T wire firmly while stripping PTFE with smooth tweezers www.emsdiasum.com |
Tweezers, High Precision, Smooth, 4 3/4 in | Electron Microscopy Sciences | 78313-00B | www.emsdiasum.com |
Tweezers, Ultra Fine Tips, 4 3/4 in | Electron Microscopy Sciences | 78510-0 | To strip 1 mm of PTFE from one end of 3T wire; grasp shielded portion with smooth tweezers www.emsdiasum.com |
Wire Stripper, 16-26 AWG | Any | Use the blade end to cut micro strips | |
Name | Company | Catalog Number | Comments |
Eyelid Surgery | |||
Surgical Instruments (High Quality Stainless Steel) | |||
2 x Dressing Forceps, 4 in Serrated | Biomedical Research Instruments | 30-1205 | Can be substituted; extra forceps for grasping electrodes/screws outside of surgery tray brisurgical.com |
Dressing Forceps, 3 in Serrated | Biomedical Research Instruments | 30-1200 | Can be substituted brisurgical.com |
Instrument Tray | Biomedical Research Instruments | 24-1355 | Can be substituted brisurgical.com |
Knife Handle No. 3, 5 in | Biomedical Research Instruments | 26-1000 | Can be substituted brisurgical.com |
Micro Dissecting Forceps, 3.5 in, Fine Points | Biomedical Research Instruments | 10-1630 | Can be substituted brisurgical.com |
Micro Dissecting Forceps, 3.5 in, Smooth Platform (0.3 x 5 mm) | Biomedical Research Instruments | 10-1720 | brisurgical.com |
Micro Dissecting Scissors, 3.5 in, Extremely Delicate, 15 mm Blades | Biomedical Research Instruments | 11-2000 | Can be substituted brisurgical.com |
Plain Splinter Forceps, 3.5 in | Biomedical Research Instruments | 30-1600 | Can be substituted brisurgical.com |
#10 Stainless Steel Surgical Blade for #3 Handle, Sterile | Any | Can be substituted | |
0-80 x 0.125 in Stainless Steel Screws | Plastics One, Inc. | 0-80 x 0.125 | Can be substituted www.plastics1.com |
Alcohol Prep Pads, Sterile | Fisher Scientific | 22-363-750 [Fisher Scientific | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Betadine Povidone-Iodine | Purdue Frederick Co. | 6761815101 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Betadine Povidone-Iodine Prep Pads | Moore Medical | 19-898-946 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Cotton-Tipped Swabs, Autoclavable | Any | Typically 7.6 cm or 15.2 cm length | |
Drill Bit for Pin Vise, #55 (0.052 in) | Any | Metal should resist rusting and corrosion | |
Gauze Pads, 2 in x 2 in | Fisher Scientific | 22-362-178 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
General Purpose Latex/Nitrile/Vinyl Gloves | Any | ||
Glass Bead Sterilizer | Any | Sterilize instruments between surgeries | |
Heated Water Therapy Pump w/ Pads x 2 | Gaymar | TP-500 | Can be substituted; separate pumps are recommended – 1 for surgery, 1 for recovery |
Hypodermic Needles 26G x 3/8 in, Sterile | Any | ||
Isoflurane | Vedco | NDC 50989-150-12 | Manfacturer can be substituted; veterinary approval may be required |
Isoflurane Vaporizer System, Tabletop, Non-Rebreathing | Parkland Scientific | V3000PK | Can be substituted www.parklandscientific.com |
Jewelers Screwdriver w/ 1.8-2 mm Blade | Any | Metal should resist rusting and corrosion | |
Ortho-Jet BCA Package (Dental Cement) | Lang Dental | B1334 | Contains powder (1 lb) and liquid www.langdental.com |
Oxygen Tank with Pressure Regulator, Large | Local supplier | ||
Porcelain Crucible, High-Form, Glazed, 10 ml | CoorsTek, Inc. | 07-965C [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted with Fisher FB-965-I Wide-Form Crucible www.fishersci.com |
Puralube Veterinary Ophthalmic Ointment, Sterile | Henry Schein Company | NC0144682 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Quatricide PV-15 | Pharmacal | PV-15 | Antimicrobial disinfectant; can be substituted www.pharmacal.com |
Rat Gas Anesthesia Masks for Stereotaxic Surgery | Stoelting Company | 51610 | www.stoeltingco.com |
Rat Stereotaxic Apparatus w/ Ear Bars (45 Degree) | Any | 45 degree bars are recommended to prevent damaging eardrums | |
Roboz Surgical Instrument Milk | Roboz Surgical | NC9358575 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted; for lubricating instruments during autoclaving www.fishersci.com |
Rodent Hair Trimmer | Any | ||
Sodium Chloride | Fisher Scientific | S641-500 [Fisher Scientific] | To make 0.9% saline; reagent grade; USP www.fishersci.com |
Stainless Steel Microspatula (Blade: 0.75 L x 0.18 in. W) | Fisher Scientific | 21-401-15 [Fisher Scientific] | Can be substituted www.fishersci.com |
Starrett Pin Vise, 0.000 in – 0.055 in | Any | Nickel-plated or equivalent recommended to resist rusting and corrosion | |
Sterile Surgical Gloves | Any | ||
Sterilization Wraps, 20 in x 20 in, Autoclavable | Propper Manufacturing | 11-890-8C [Fisher Scientific] | Useful for wrapping autoclavable supplies and on sterile field during surgery www.fishersci.com |
Surgical Drape, Sterile/Autoclavable | Any | May need to cut to size for rats | |
Surgical Gown* | Any | *If required by IACUC | |
Surgical Mask | Any | ||
Tuberculin Syringes, Sterile, 1.0 ml | Any | ||
Weigh Scale | Any | Should have good resolution (in gram units) | |
Name | Company | Catalog Number | Comments |
Eyeblink System and Components (assuming 4-rodent system) | |||
5 Channel Commutator x 4 | Plastics One, Inc. | SL2 + 3C | www.plastics1.com |
Bipolar Electrode Cable, Dual 305 x 4 | Plastics One, Inc. | 305-305 80CM TT2 (C) | Provides plug end to bipolar electrode on rat and to commutator; must be modified www.plastics1.com |
Cable, 5 Channel, Shielded, 26 AWG x 4 | Any | To fabricate commutator cable; this must be made from scratch | |
Chamber for Operant Test Box (Inside: 24 H x 23 W x 14 D in) x 4 | Med-Associates | Can be substituted; inner dimensions should fit operant test box comfortably, with room for acoustical foam; fit with fan – 55-60 dB www.med-associates.com |
|
Eyeblink System and Software | JSA Designs | N/A | Proprietary and customized for research lab |
Heat Shrink Tubing (3/16 in, 1/4 in, 3/8 in, 1/2 in Diameters) | Any | To protect modified commutator cable soldered ends and splices | |
Melamine Triple Peak Acoustical Foam w/Black Hypalon (24 x 48 in) | McMaster-Carr | 9162T5 | Can be substituted; cut to fit 4 housing chambers www.mcmaster.com |
Operant Test Box (Exterior 12.5 L x 10 W x 13.5 in H), Complete x 4 | Med-Associates | ENV-007 Custom Package | With stainless steel grid floor and custom top (3 in hole in center for commutator cable) www.med-associates.com |
Oscilloscope (Optional) | Any | Recommended minimum specs: 200 MHz analog bandwidth, 1 GS/s real-time sampling, 4 channels; see www.picotech.com /td> |
|
Piezo Tweeters (Speakers) x 4 (7 x 3 in) | MCM Electronics | 53-805 | Must match frequency range specifications for eyeblink system (2500 Hz – 25 KHz) www.mcmelectronics.com |
Soldering Station, Solder, Flux, Tinner | Any | For soldering 26 AWG cables to female sockets (that fit male relia-tac contact pins) and bipolar plugs | |
Stimulus Isolators x 4 | WPI International | A365 | These units run on 16-9V alkaline batteries; a suitable rechargeable version (A365R) is available www.wpiinc.com |
Tripolar Electrode Cable for SL3C Commutator x 4 | Plastics One, Inc. | 335-335 80cm TT3 C | Provides plug end to EMG headstage on rat and to commutator; must be modified www.plastics1.com |
USB LED Lights x 4 | Any | USB-based lights do not cause electrical "noise" with the EMG signals from the rats www.plastics1.com |
|
Webcams x 4, Surveillance Software | Any | ||
PC Computer Running MS Windows OS | Any |