This protocol aims to investigate the neural activity related to social comparison and social distance during the processing of intertemporal decision outcomes. Indifference points will be measured using event-related potentials as part of the study.
Intertemporal choice plays a crucial role in our daily lives, influencing decisions related to education, health, consumption, and investment. This research proposes an innovative experimental protocol that examines how social comparison and social distance jointly affect the neural processes involved in outcome assessment for intertemporal choices. The study is based on the theoretical framework of cognitive resource competition. This protocol enables researchers to dynamically establish an indifference point for each participant, effectively eliminating the influence of any biased indifference points on the assessment of intertemporal choices. Consequently, the study solely measures the combined impact of social comparison and social distance on how participants evaluate intertemporal choice outcomes. The findings reveal that individuals are more inclined to opt for immediate outcomes under negative unfair conditions. Moreover, compared to the fair and positive unfair conditions, people tend to undervalue delayed outcomes in the negative unfair condition. The strength of this approach lies in its dynamic indifference point setting, making it an effective method to investigate the influence of various external factors (such as social status and power level) on intertemporal decision-making. While the protocol is designed to measure electrophysiological events like event-related potentials, it can also be tailored for use with fMRI.
In daily life, people often face the choice of enjoying the present or investing in the future. This decision, known as intertemporal choice, requires individuals to weigh the value of outcomes at different time points1,2,3. Over time, the subjective evaluation of outcomes declines hyperbolically or quasi-hyperbolically4,5,6,7. People tend to prefer small-but-immediate gains over larger-but-later ones8.
Previous research has examined various factors influencing intertemporal decision-making. For instance, D. Wang et al.9 explored self-other differences in intertemporal decision-making and found that decisions made for oneself or friends tend to prefer delayed larger rewards over immediate smaller rewards compared to decisions made for strangers. The closeness of social relationships affects individuals' perception of time, thereby impacting their intertemporal decision-making. Similarly, Zhao et al.10 conducted experiments on self-other decision-making in time-based intertemporal choice. The results revealed that participants tend to opt for the smaller immediate option when making decisions for others, but they prefer the large-but-later option for themselves, highlighting the influence of personal interests on the intertemporal decision-making process.
While previous studies have focused on the behavioral and psychological aspects of intertemporal decision-making, they have not provided a direct understanding of the cognitive process or an in-depth analysis of the underlying neural mechanisms. However, an increasing number of recent studies have employed the event-related potentials (ERPs) method to explore intertemporal decision-making and its neurocognitive processes. ERPs refer to the measured brain responses resulting from specific sensory, cognitive, or motor events11. The use of ERPs offers two significant advantages for studying intertemporal decision-making. Firstly, its high temporal resolution enables the differentiation of the temporal sequence of different cognitive processes. Secondly, ERPs components can serve as indicators of specific cognitive processes. For instance, H. Y. Zhang et al.12 utilized ERPs to investigate the effect of social distance on outcome comparison between individuals and their partners. They concluded that personal closeness moderates individuals' subjective sensitivity during the outcome comparison phase. The study also found that participants expressed greater satisfaction with the loss outcomes of dislikeable players. ERPs components were employed to analyze influential cognitive processes, showing that this higher satisfaction with loss outcomes for dislikeable players resulted from subjects' increased sensitivity to evaluative, motivational, and emotional processes involved in social comparison.
Previous studies have primarily focused on the competition for cognitive resources between immediate and delayed options in intertemporal decision-making. However, the brain simultaneously processes various tasks, including social comparison and social distance, which further competes for limited cognitive resources. As a result, fewer cognitive resources are allocated to the intertemporal decision-making task. To accurately investigate the influence of external factors on intertemporal decision outcomes, it is crucial to identify the equilibrium state of cognitive resource allocation between immediate and delayed outcome evaluation. In the equilibrium state, individuals assign the same subjective value to the delayed outcome as they do to the immediate outcome. However, if external factors, such as social comparison and social distance, are given more weight in the equilibrium state, it disrupts the cognitive resource balance in intertemporal decision-making. As a result, the cognitive difference between the equilibrium and inequilibrium state can precisely reflect the impact of external factors on the evaluation of intertemporal decision results. The "indifference point" represents the balance point of the delayed outcome on a fixed date in the future, equivalent to the subjective value of the immediate outcome13. Some existing studies on intertemporal decision-making have not set the indifference point for each participant in their experimental paradigm. Instead, they calculate the subject's time discount rate in advance using a delay discount task (DDT) and categorize participants into high and low time discount rate groups. Consequently, the results of studies exploring external factors influencing intertemporal decision-making become inconsistent due to the imbalanced allocation of cognitive resources between immediate and delayed option evaluation14,15,16.
Only a limited number of studies have explored the combined influence of social comparison and social distance on individuals' intertemporal decision-making, and even fewer have utilized the ERPs technique. Consequently, the underlying neural mechanism of intertemporal choice outcome evaluation in the presence of both social factors remains unclear. Existing studies on the impact of external factors on intertemporal decision-making have suffered from inadequate setting of indifference points for delayed outcomes, leading to potential deviations in measuring the effect of these external factors. Different individuals may assign different subjective value evaluations to the same amount of rewards, necessitating the setting of individualized indifference points for each participant to eliminate interference caused by inequitable cognitive resource allocation during intertemporal outcome evaluation. A new experimental paradigm, in which the indifference point for delayed outcomes is determined in advance, is essential to address this issue. A previous study proposed such a paradigm with a fixed one-month delay outcome indifference point, yielding results consistent with expectations from cognitive resource competition theory17. Though setting an indifference point in advance may introduce bias, it can still effectively influence participants through psychological cues and cognitive reinforcement.
In contrast to prior research where participants merely observed intertemporal choices without direct personal involvement, the current study presents a novel experimental paradigm. Participants are not only engaged in the gambling task but also required to compare their outcomes with others who transition from being strangers to friends. This paradigm explores both individual self-interest in intertemporal choices and the cognitive processing of social comparison, significantly differing from previous investigations. By having participants report their indifference points for one-month delayed outcomes in the DDT task and subsequently using these self-reported indifference points as the delay option's outcome in the upcoming intertemporal decision-making task, this study aims to provide a pure measurement of the joint influence of social comparison and social distance on outcome evaluation in intertemporal decision-making, assuming no glitches occur during the indifference point setting process.
People not only need to perceive interpersonal relationships but also engage in social comparison by comparing their outcomes with others. However, it is unclear whether the interpersonal perceptual task and the social comparison task consume cognitive resources independently or compete for resources during the integrated assessment of the time value of intertemporal choice outcomes. The N100 is a negative deflected brain wave occurring within a 100 ms time window after an event, considered an indicator of attention distribution before comprehensive outcome evaluation. Its amplitude decreases as the number of attention resources increases18. Liu et al.19 found a significant effect of social distance during the early N100 stage of outcome processing, suggesting that individuals tend to compare themselves with close people on the ability dimension during the primary stage of outcome processing. Additionally, Mason et al.20 argued that subjects exhibited more negative N100 amplitudes in response to immediate rewards compared to delayed rewards, indicating that temporal delay is encoded in early neural processing.
The P300 is a positive deflected brain wave appearing around 300 ms after an event, serving as a direct index of outcome evaluation. A larger P300 amplitude indicates a higher attentional allocation and more exhaustive outcome assessment12. H. Y. Zhang et al.12 demonstrated that the P300 was larger during the outcome evaluation phase of gambling with dislikeable players, reflecting participants' stronger motivation to outperform dislikeable players. Moreover, anxious individuals may struggle with concentrating or focusing on anything beyond their present worries due to their avoidance of future uncertainties21. An ERP study on the influence of anxiety levels on intertemporal choice outcomes showed that highly anxious individuals exhibited a significantly more positive P300 amplitude when seeing the immediate option compared to the delayed option22. According to resource allocation theory, cognitive resources allocated to the intertemporal decision-making task are reduced during the comprehensive outcome assessment phase. Hypothesis 1 proposes competition for cognitive resources between the interpersonal perceptual task, social comparison task, and intertemporal decision-making task at different cognitive stages. At the electrophysiological level, there are main or interaction effects for social distance and time delay on the N100 component, and for social comparison and time delay on the P300 component.
Based on cognitive resource competition theory, when additional tasks such as social comparison and interpersonal perceptual tasks are introduced, they compete for limited cognitive resources with the intertemporal decision-making task. As a result, fewer cognitive resources are available for the intertemporal decision-making task, leading to a lack of elaborate processing of the time effect on outcome evaluation. This results in individuals having a reduced sensitivity to time and a smaller time discounting rate. In light of this theory, hypothesis 2 is proposed for the present study: when participants face both social comparison and interpersonal perceptual tasks simultaneously, they will have a higher evaluation for delayed outcomes. Specifically, compared to immediate rewards, delayed rewards will elicit a more positive P300 amplitude at the EEG level. This effect is expected due to the increased competition for cognitive resources, leading to a stronger attentional allocation and more exhaustive evaluation of delayed outcomes.
According to D. Kahneman23, attention is divisible, and the allocation of attention is a matter of degree. When faced with multiple parallel tasks, individuals prioritize them based on their relevance to self-interest and allocate cognitive resources accordingly24. However, numerous studies have indicated that an inferior task with limited cognitive resources may be susceptible to interference and have negligible effects on other tasks. This could be due to a significant disparity in cognitive resource allocation between tasks of different priorities. In the present experimental paradigm, the intertemporal decision-making task is considered a superior task directly linked to self-interest, thus receiving the highest priority in cognitive resource allocation. Compared to the social comparison task and the interpersonal perceptual task, the intertemporal decision-making task is allocated cognitive resources at least one order of magnitude higher. Hypothesis 3 proposes that despite the simultaneous processing of the social comparison task and the interpersonal perceptual task, individuals will rate the immediate and delayed outcomes equally. This means that there will be no significant difference in the P300 component of neural activities between immediate and delayed outcome conditions. This hypothesis is based on the premise that the intertemporal decision-making task receives significantly more cognitive resources due to its higher priority, making the cognitive resource competition between immediate and delayed outcomes less pronounced. As a result, individuals would evaluate the two outcomes equally at the neural activity level.
When individuals perceive that their reward is less than what others receive, they often experience feelings of dissatisfaction and anger. This realization can motivate them to either seek changes in the current situation or withdraw from comparisons altogether to establish a perceived sense of fairness25. In an unfairly disadvantaged circumstance, a significant disparity in rewards can negatively impact an individual's self-esteem, leading them to avoid comparing themselves with others and redirecting cognitive resources to a less challenging task26. As a psychological defense mechanism, individuals facing an unfair disadvantage comparison condition will reallocate cognitive resources from the social comparison task to the intertemporal decision-making task. Higher time discounting rates are associated with greater cognitive resource allocation. Based on the above understanding, the present article proposes hypothesis 4: in comparison to both fair or unfair advantage conditions, subjects will assign lower evaluations to delayed rewards in the unfair disadvantage condition. At the electrophysiological level, this is expected to be reflected in a smaller P300 component elicited by delayed rewards in the unfair disadvantaged condition. This effect occurs due to the reallocation of cognitive resources to the intertemporal decision-making task, leading to reduced attentional allocation and a less exhaustive evaluation of delayed outcomes.
In the context of the unfairly disadvantaged circumstance, the reallocation of increased cognitive resources to the intertemporal decision-making task may not significantly impact the assessment of immediate outcomes. This is because the immediate outcome's time value may not require extensive processing, leading to a lesser influence of cognitive resource reallocation on this aspect. Therefore, hypothesis 5 is proposed, suggesting that in the unfairly disadvantaged circumstance, people are more likely to opt for an immediate reward. At the neural activity level, there will be a distinct difference in the P300 component between the immediate and delayed outcomes due to the different sensitivity of time perception.
Additionally, when individuals are engaged in a gambling task with a friend and face an unfair outcome, fewer cognitive resources will be allocated to the evaluation of the intertemporal choice's outcome due to the demands of perceiving and processing social relationships. Consequently, as a result of reduced cognitive resources, individuals become less sensitive to time in this situation. Hence, hypothesis 6 is raised: compared to interactions with strangers, people will express more satisfaction with delayed rewards in the unfairly disadvantaged condition. This means that delayed rewards will produce a larger P300 component at the neural activity level in the context of interactions with friends compared to strangers.
This research scheme was approved by the local and institutional ethics committee and complies with the latest version of the Declaration of Helsinki. All participants provided written informed consent before participating. The participants had normal vision or normal correction and no psychiatric or neurological disorders. The participants did not have drug or psychotropic medication using experience and no perm or hair dyeing history within six months. If subjects had excessive artifacts in the EEG data, they were not included in the subsequent data analysis.
1. Experimental stimuli
Small Fair condition | Negative Unfair condition | Positive Unfair condition | Large Fair Condition | |
Now | 10 Vs 10 | 10 Vs 20 | 20 Vs 10 | 20 Vs 20 |
1 month | X1 Vs X1 | X1 Vs X2 | X2 Vs X1 | X2 Vs X2 |
Table 1: The collection of gambling results. The table depicts the set of 8 social comparison results.
Figure 1: Stimuli of the feedback interface for gambling task. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
2. Experimental procedure
Figure 2: The process of the Delay Discounting Task (DDT). Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 3: Time course of a single trial. The figure depicts the procedure of a single trial for the gambling task. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
3. Experimental preparation and electrophysiological recording
IOS scale result
The IOS scale score30 was used to examine the social distance and self-relevance of the participants to friends and strangers, and it was found that the social distance of the participants to friends (6.20 ± 0.696) was higher than the social distance of the participants to strangers (1.45 ± 0.605), t(19) = 21.978, p < 0.001, 95%, Cl = (4.30 – 5.20), revealing that the social distance manipulation is effective.
Behavioral result
A paired-sample t-test was performed on the average reaction time of the participants. The statistical results showed that there was no significant difference in reaction time between the friend condition (675.830 ± 117.295 ms) and the stranger condition (688.381 ± 113.012 ms), t(17) = -0.840, p = 0.412 > 0.05.
EEG results
EEG results of N100
The following figures are the waveform diagram (Figure 4) and topographic map (Figure 5) of the N100 component. A 2 (social distance: friends vs. strangers) × 2 (time delay: now vs. 1 month) × 4 (social comparison: small fair, large fair, negative unfair, positive unfair) × 6 (electrodes: F3, FZ, F4, FC3, FCZ, FC4) repeated measures analysis of variance on the N100 component was performed. The results showed that the main effect of social distance was significant (F(1,17) = 4.962,p = 0.040, = 0.226). The amplitude of N100 caused by friends (-3.130 ± 0.463 μV) was more negative than by strangers (-2.742 ± 0.470 μV,p = 0.040). In addition, the main effect of time delay was significant (F(1,17) = 5.341,p = 0.034, = 0.239). The N100 amplitude caused by the immediate outcome condition (-3.116 ± 0.447 μV) was more negative than the delayed outcome condition (-2.756 ± 0.482 μV,p = 0.034) (Figure 6). The N100 component did not have the main effects of social comparison (p = 0.969).
NOTE: The steps of EEG data analysis and the parameter settings will be shown in the form of a video.
Figure 4: ERP waveform at FCZ channel for each condition. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 5: The topographic map at 130 ms for each condition. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 6: The amplitude of N100 under different conditions. n = 18, the error bar reflects the standard error. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
EEG results of P300
The waveforms of the P300 components are shown in Figure 7. A 2 (social distance: friends vs. strangers) × 2 (time delay: now vs. 1 month) × 4 (social comparison: small fair, large fair, negative unfair, positive unfair) × 6 (Electrodes: CP3, CPZ, CP4, P3, PZ, P4) repeated measures analysis of variance on the P300 component was also performed. The results showed that the main effect of social comparison was significantly different at the 0.1 level (F(1.759,29.897) = 3.011,p = 0.070, = 0.150). The results of the post-pairwise comparison reveal that the better outcome of positive unfair (4.170 ± 0.461 μV,p = 0.004) and the equal outcome of large fair (4.092 ± 0.476 μV,p = 0.002) caused P300 were more positive than the worse outcome of negative unfair (3.392 ± 0.456 μV). The difference between the first two was not significant (Figure 8).
Figure 7: ERP waveform at CPZ channel for each condition. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 8: P300 amplitude of four comparison conditions. n = 18, the error bar highlights the standard error. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
In addition, it was also found that the social comparison × time delay interaction was significant at the 0.1 level (F(3,51) = 2.198,p = 0.100, = 0.114). Further simple effect analysis demonstrated that when the result was a worse outcome of negative unfair condition, the P300 caused by immediate gain (3.927 ± 0.501 μV) was greater than delayed gain (2.856 ± 0.541 μV,p = 0.049). Other results did not induce similar effects. When the outcome was delayed, the P300 components of small fair condition (3.950 ± 0.570 μV,p = 0.023), positive unfair condition (4.024 ± 0.590 μV,p = 0.022) and large fair condition (4.440 ± 0.505 μV,p = 0.004) were more positive than the worse outcome of negative unfair (2.856 ± 0.541 μV), there was no significant effect between these three conditions. However, no effect was found when the results were gained immediately (Figure 9). The following is the topographic map of the brain at the maximum latency in the time window of the P300 component (Figure 10).
Figure 9: The amplitude of P300 under different conditions. n = 18, the error bar shows the standard error. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 10: The topographic map at 400 ms for each condition. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Experimental results and significance
Generally, additional tasks, such as social distance perception and social comparison, compete for cognitive resources with the intertemporal decision-making task at different cognitive stages. Firstly, both social distance and time delay have main effects on N100 amplitude, respectively. The present results indicate that gambling with friends induces a greater N100 amplitude than with strangers. Moreover, immediate outcomes elicit greater N100 amplitude than delayed outcomes, suggesting that cognitive resource competition occurs at an earlier stage, specifically the stage of attention resource allocation represented by N100, rather than during outcome evaluation between interpersonal perceptual and intertemporal decision-making tasks. Consistent with some previous findings, this study's results suggest that individuals' processing of social distance precedes outcome assessment, depleting the cognitive resources used for intertemporal choice outcome assessment18,31. However, the study did not show any interaction effects between the two, indicating that while the interpersonal perceptual task depletes cognitive resources from the intertemporal decision-making task, social distance does not impact the assessment of intertemporal choice outcomes. Secondly, there was an interaction effect between social comparison and time delay on P300, indicating that cognitive resources are simultaneously consumed in the comprehensive assessment process indexed by P300 in both social comparison and intertemporal decision-making tasks. Thus, the social comparison task significantly influences the outcome assessment of the intertemporal decision-making task32,33,34,35. Prior research has shown that individuals' assessment of intertemporal choice outcomes involves at least two stages: the processing of time and the comprehensive assessment of the outcome36,37,38,39. The results of this study are consistent with these findings.
According to the experimental results, there was no significant difference in the P300 amplitude between the immediate and delayed outcomes. Hypothesis 3 was confirmed, while hypothesis 2 was falsified. A reliable explanation is that, instead of allocating cognitive resources quantitatively to each task indistinguishably, individuals prioritize tasks based on the relevance of self-interests primarily24. The task of evaluating the results of intertemporal choices is most directly related to self-interests and is the primary task for individuals, while the task of comparing results with others is secondary. As a result, the primary task allocates more cognitive resources than the secondary task. Therefore, even when the secondary task is cognitively processed, it does not affect the outcome assessment of the primary task.
Further evidence suggests that the P300 amplitude of the delayed outcome in the negative unfair condition was smaller than in the other three conditions; however, the immediate outcome did not exhibit this significant difference. Specifically, the P300 amplitude of the immediate outcomes in the negative unfair condition was larger than that of the delayed outcomes; this significant difference was not observed in the other three conditions. When individuals are faced with negative unfair conditions, avoiding the disadvantageous situation allows for the transfer of cognitive resources originally devoted to the social comparison task to the intertemporal decision-making task. This reallocation of cognitive resources results in an increased focus on time perception, making the processing of delayed outcomes more detailed and leading to a more fine-grained assessment of outcomes in the negative unfair condition. Consequently, delayed outcomes receive worse evaluations compared to the fair and positive unfair conditions.
Moreover, as there is no need for an additional temporal discounting of the immediate outcome, even an increase in cognitive resources would not impact the assessment of the immediate outcome. On the other hand, for the evaluation of the delayed outcome, the increased cognitive resources are further invested in processing the time value of the outcome, leading to a higher discount rate for individuals. This would explain the lower evaluation of the delayed outcome than the immediate outcome under the negative unfair condition.
As D. Kahneman23 and his followers suggest, if two tasks cannot maintain high-level performance at the same time, they may share the same cognitive resource pool. Otherwise, they are driven by different resources, respectively. It is challenging to disprove this theory since the source and destination of cognitive resources cannot be accurately identified with existing technical tools. In this study, a new ingenious experimental paradigm was implemented to challenge the above theory. Generally, the additional social comparison task seizes partial cognitive resources from the intertemporal decision-making task. However, in the unfair disadvantage condition, people are emotionally inclined to evade the embarrassing results in the social comparison task due to impaired self-esteem. As a result, the resources previously occupied by the social comparison task are retrieved intact and redirected to the intertemporal decision-making task. Theoretically, the amount of cognitive resources used for the intertemporal decision-making task in the unfair disadvantage conditions is roughly the same as when solely carrying out the intertemporal decision-making task without distractions. Based on cognitive resource allocation theory, when the same amount of cognitive resources is used, the same evaluation of intertemporal choice outcomes is expected.
However, the results of the present study showed an astonishing difference, with P300 elicited by delayed rewards even smaller than for immediate rewards. A plausible explanation is that abundant cognitive resources are tied up in the primary superior task, initiating a top-down cognitive processing mode that drives people to focus more on the task's frame, such as gain/loss, than on specific details. As a result, vacant cognitive resources for secondary tasks become too scarce to initiate a top-down cognitive pattern, leading to a detail-oriented bottom-up pattern. However, when cognitive resources are returned from the secondary inferior task to the primary superior task, due to cognitive inertia, they are still carried out in an extra bottom-up cognitive processing mode, causing people to pay more attention to the details of the primary superior task40, which, in this study, is time perception. Consequently, besides the amount of cognitive resources, the change of cognitive processing mode will also affect the cognitive effect. Even if the exact same cognitive resources are used in the same cognitive task, they will show different cognitive effects in different cognitive processing modes.
EEG evidence from the present study showed no significant difference in the P300 amplitude produced by immediate outcomes and delayed outcomes when there was a negative unfair outcome in the gambling task with a friend. That is, social distance does not affect the outcome assessment of intertemporal decision-making, which is consistent with the findings revealed by Jin et al.24. This is because individuals' interpersonal perceptions of gambling opponents have no direct relevance to their interests and should be the lowest priority task, for which even fewer cognitive resources are allocated than for the social comparison task. The interpersonal perception of gambling opponents barely preempts cognitive resources for the intertemporal decision-making task. The present study provides an explanatory framework for self-execution from the perspective of cognitive resource allocation.
Effectiveness of the method
The prime aim of the present protocol is to propose a new two-stage paradigm to explore the joint effect of social comparison and social distance on the evaluation of intertemporal choice outcomes. In the first stage, a DDT task is executed to rapidly measure the indifference points for each individual, which is used to assign the delayed reward outcomes and eliminate different time value evaluations between immediate outcomes and delayed outcomes. Given that there are no subjective valuation differences between immediate rewards and delayed rewards, the cognitive process of these two different intertemporal choice outcomes is equal. Therefore, the measurement after eliminating the interference of indifference point deflection accurately reflects the influence of external factors (i.e., social distance, social comparison, etc.) on the outcome evaluation of intertemporal decision-making.
To verify the present protocol's validity, it is necessary to first examine whether there is a difference in the evaluation of the immediate and delayed outcomes designed with indifference points. Previous research has indicated that P300 is used to express the outcome evaluation process and is able to distinguish between positive and negative outcomes. Specifically, the positive outcome caused a more positive P300 amplitude35. The current results demonstrate that there is no significant difference between the immediate gain condition and the delayed gain condition (Figure 9).
The further argument pertains to whether social comparison and social distance jointly influence the process of evaluating intertemporal choice outcomes. The present study suggests that the N100 effect shows signs of abating in response to social distance and time delay, respectively. As noted earlier, processing social distance information and time-delayed information will preempt limited shared cognitive resources. This means that a suboptimal detailed evaluation of intertemporal choice outcomes is applied due to the shortage of cognitive resources. This further evidence clarifies that social comparison and social distance can mediate the evaluation of intertemporal choice outcomes. A reliable explanation is that the distraction of other tasks will decrease the sensitivity of time perception, leading to a neglect of time, and eventually a reduction in their time discount rate.
The present method has been demonstrated to be effective in manipulating the indifference points of intertemporal decision-making tasks while exploring the combined influence of social comparison and social distance. Consequently, this protocol is a helpful approach to investigate not only the joint effect of social comparison and social distance but also the influence of other external factors, such as social status and social power, on the evaluation of intertemporal choice outcomes.
Further explanations and other applications
This study provides an effective method to explore the evaluation of outcomes in intertemporal decision-making. To eliminate the influence of intertemporal decision-making itself, a DDT task was designed to determine individuals' indifference points. Based on these indifference points, the feedback interface of the gambling task was designed. Therefore, constructing subjects' indifference points in the experiment is a key step in this protocol. On the one hand, since each subject's indifference points are set based on their time preference, the influence of individual trait differences on intertemporal decision-making can be eliminated. On the other hand, removing the influence of the intertemporal choice factors, such as time and the number of rewards, provides a pure method to measure the combined influence of social comparison and social distance on evaluating intertemporal choice outcomes.
This method can inform future research related to the evaluation of outcomes in intertemporal decision-making. Exploring the cognitive processes associated with outcome assessment is widely used in intertemporal decision-making research. However, most studies have not eliminated the effects of individual differences and the influence of intertemporal decision-making, and this protocol provides a method to address and improve these limitations.
Addressing the limitation
Further improvements can be made to the methodology of this study. For instance, the results of the social comparison in this study were all based on the same time span. Future studies could consider adding comparisons of results from different time spans to explore potential variations in outcome evaluations based on varying timeframes. Additionally, this research exclusively focused on the processing of intertemporal outcomes under the gain situation due to the sign effect. It would be worthwhile to investigate further whether this effect also exists under the loss situation. Incorporating these enhancements into the study's methodology would undoubtedly contribute to expanding our understanding of how social comparison and social distance jointly influence the evaluation of intertemporal choice outcomes.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
This work was supported by the project of National Natural Science Foundation of China (72001055), the project of Social Science Foundation of Heilongjiang Province of China (18JLC219), the project of Postdoctoral Foundation of Heilongjiang Province of China (LBH-Z18018), the project of Scholars Plan of Northeast Agricultural University (2019), and the Philosophy and Social Science Research Project of Jiangsu Provincial Department of Education (2018SJA1089).
We thank all colleagues in Lab 412, especially Zhikai Song and Xinyue Jia, for their assistance in the experiment. We would also like to thank the editors and anonymous reviewers for their valuable suggestions.
Electrode caps | Neurosoft Labs, Inc, USA | 64 Ag/AgCl electrodes | with a configuration of the international 10–20 system of electrode |
E-Prime software | Psychology Software Tools, Inc, USA | 2 | Experimental generation system for computerized behavior research |
Liquid Crystal Display Monitor | ROYAL PHILIPS, Netherlands | Display experimental procedure | |
NeuroScan Synamp2 Amplifier | Neurosoft Labs, Inc, USA | bandpass filter 0.05-100 Hz, sampling rate 1000 Hz |