This protocol outlines the use of spectrophotometry to detect ultraviolet-reflecting structures on organisms (in this example, the sailfin molly Poecilia latipinna) and describes dichotomous choice tests for fish that allow inferences to be made on the role of ultraviolet cues during mate selection.
Many organisms use cues and signals beyond human sensitivity during social interactions. It is important to take into account how organisms perceive their worlds when trying to understand their behavior and ecology. Sensitivity to the ultraviolet spectrum (UV; 300 – 400 nm) is found across multiple genera of birds, fish, reptiles, amphibians, and even mammals. This protocol describes a technique for examining organisms for the presence of UV-reflecting structures and a method for testing whether these cues are used as social signals in the context of mate choice. A spectrophotometer is used to detect the presence of UV reflectance and variation in reflective intensity between individuals and sexes. An example of this technique is presented in which a dichotomous mate choice test exposes sexually receptive individuals to opposite sex individuals whose visual appearance can be manipulated by filters that either transmit full spectrum or block UV wavelengths. This system allowed for the determination that female, but not male, sailfin mollies (Poecilia latipinna) were using UV markings as part of their mating decisions. These types of studies serve to expand our knowledge of the range of organisms that utilize UV and provide insight into how UV plays a role in their lives.
Understanding the cues and signals used in animal social interactions allows us to comprehend the phenotypic variation both within and among species. This variation plays an important role in evolutionary processes such as population divergence, sexual selection, and speciation. Often, however, researchers are limited to exploring the cues most obvious to human sensory systems, especially those within the visual or auditory realms. Use of spectrophotometry, however, allows us to expand our investigations beyond the human visible spectrum and into wavelengths that may be important in social interactions in other species.
In particular, the short-range communication afforded by ultraviolet (UV; 300 – 400 nm) sensitivity has the potential to be highly advantageous during mate choice1. Many visually-hunting predators of birds and fishes, for example, are unable to detect UV radiation. In systems where males display elaborately to females, these males would reduce their risk of predation while maintaining their ability to attract mates by exploiting the UV spectrum rather than developing cues detectable in the visible spectrum2,3.. If one fails to consider the possibility that organisms are communicating with each other using these "private communication channels", significant drivers of behavior and evolution may be missed.
This protocol outlines an investigation into the use of UV cues for mate choice in the sailfin molly, Poecilia latipinna, a polygamous fish that has no previously known ability to detect UV or utilize UV markings. This fish species has a close phylogenetic proximity to other UV-sensitive live-bearing fishes4 and there is microspectroscopic evidence that P. latipinna, along with other molly species such as P. mexicana and P. formosa, possess a class of cones (photoreceptor cells responsible for color vision) that are most sensitive for UV wavelengths5. In this sexually dimorphic species, female choice has played a strong role in the evolution of the males' brightly colored and enlarged fins6-9. This methodology allows us to explore whether UV is an additional medium by which females assess male quality.
The detection and measurement of UV markings on P. latipinna using a spectrophotometer with a fiber optic probe is detailed here. Further, whether receptive female mollies differentially associate with males viewed through an optical filter transmitting full-spectrum light including UV-A ([UV+]; 320 – 700 nm) and males viewed through an UV-blocking filter ([UV-]; 400 – 700 nm) is discussed. This method has broad applications for discovering UV sensitivity and color patterns in fishes and other organisms, allowing research into a variety of questions involving UV and its role in behavior.
All experiments were conducted with the approval of Ohio Wesleyan University's Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee.
1. Recording UV Reflectance of Fish using a Spectrophotometer
2. Dichotomous Mating Trials
NOTE: Perform control observations, in which focal fish, UV filters, and opaque filters are in place. In these trials, when the opaque filters are removed, focal fish will be exposed to parts of the tank displaying the presence and absence of UV light, but lacking potential mating partners. For details on the validity of dichotomous choice tests in this experiment see Palmer and Hankison (2015)10, although note that these tests also have drawbacks11.
Figure 1. Experimental Aquarium Setup. A rectangular aquarium divided into three sections: a central area that held the focal individual and two end compartments that held object pairs (the figure shows a female in the middle with object males at the ends). Interchangeable filters that either blocked or permitted UV reflectance could be placed over the glass partitions dividing the male and female sections of the aquarium. This figure is modified from Palmer and Hankison (2015) 10. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figures 1 and 2 show the mating preference aquarium set up and UV measurement sites for our experiments.
Measuring UV reflectance allowed for the determination that P. latipinna do possess UV characteristics, especially along the sides of their bodies (Figure 3), in addition to individual variation in these traits. Once UV traits were found, testing revealed that female, but not male, P. latipinna use these characteristics in their mating decisions (Female: t15 = 4.08, p = 0.001; Male: t14 = 0.67, p = 0.517; Figure 4). That both sexes possess UV traits, but that the traits are used only by females as part of mate preference may indicate a role of these traits in other social interactions, such as shoaling or foraging, in P. latipinna.
Figure 2. Locations on the Poecilia latipinna that were Measured for UV Reflectance. Regions were initially determined from preliminary reflectance trials with reflectance samples taken from across nearly all body regions. 1 – 3, DF (dorsal fin) and CF (caudal fin) refer to body regions shown in Figure 3. This figure is modified from Palmer and Hankison (2015)10. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 3. Mean Spectra of Male and Female Poecilia latipinna. The graphs show the mean spectral reflectance measured at five locations (side areas, caudal fin, dorsal fin) SE. The areas inside the boxes represent reflectance in the UV spectrum. This figure is modified from Palmer and Hankison (2015)10. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4. Mean Associate Times of Males and Females. The graphs show the mean time SE that females and males spent near male compartments viewed through UV+ and UV- filters during the control and experimental tests. This figure is modified from Palmer and Hankison (2015)10. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Spectrophotometry was successful in identifying UV markings on P. latipinna. Both sexes of P. latipinna possess UV markings along their sides. In addition, some males had UV markings on their dorsal fins, traits previously found to be important in female mating preferences7.
We recommend using UV spectrophotometry as a mechanism to detect the presence of UV markings. Further testing could determine its role in social interactions, including mating preference (as detailed here). Alternatively, UV characteristics might influence shoaling, individual, sex, or species recognition, or intrasexual interactions3, 12-15.
Despite the usefulness of spectrophotometry in better understanding the UV characteristics of organisms, especially related to their social behavior, there are some critical steps necessary to fully understand the behaviors, and some limitations to what can be concluded. All experiments should test whether focal individuals have preference for UV+ or UV– environments by including controls with no test individuals in the flanking choice tanks, especially as blocking UV may cause changes in the "brightness" or luminance the organisms observe. While luminance has not been shown to influence female preference in species closely related to P. latipinna16-18 additional data detailing the role of brightness and preference tests was addressed in a previous study19 and could be an important step in ensuring that preference is not based on brightness changes. Alternatively, differences in the lighting flux (overall brightness per unit of time) can be equalized, allowing for even more control of lighting conditions in systems where this is of concern2,20.
In addition, for mate choice tests, choice individuals should be matched as closely as possible in size and coloration (aka attractiveness) so that choice more strongly reflects preference for UV+ or UV–. Alternative techniques that allow a single individual to be viewed under both UV+ and UV– may be useful for some organisms21. These studies may allow for control of any differences in individual behavior (as the same individual is viewed under UV+ and UV– conditions simultaneously). While we did not see behavioral differences among our fish in our experimental design, this aspect may be important for other organisms or experimental designs. In addition, studies that retest individual females while switching the sides of UV filters2, or that switch filters mid-way through an individual test would also be useful, both for controlling for individual side bias. Finally, while spectrophotometry and mating preference trials can reveal these aspects of an organism's phenotype, the protocol here does not have the ability to determine fully the meaning of the UV traits and what they may be revealing to focal individuals. Fitness trials to understand how UV traits are impacted by development, inheritance, environmental conditions (such as predictability of food or predators), or other factors would be useful in further understanding the meaning and role of UV characteristics.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
We are grateful to anonymous reviewers for comments and suggestions that greatly improved this manuscript. We thank R. Bowes, R. Carreno, and T. Panhuis for assistance in collecting the fish. We also thank M. Lee for assistance with male preference trials. We are grateful to the Ohio Wesleyan University Department of Zoology for helpful advice and suggestions throughout this study and Arizona State University (McGraw Lab) for software advice.
Spectrophotometer, P1000 | Ocean Optics | newer models are availabe | |
DT 1000 xenon UV light source | Ocean Optics | newer models are availabe | |
Ocean Optics Overture Software | Ocean Optics | newer software is available | |
R200-Angle-UV bifurcated fiber-optic probe (Guided Wave) | Ocean Optics | newer models are availabe | |
Certified reflectance standard, white | labsphere | ||
75.7 L Aquarium, divided | Experimental Builder | ||
Full Spectrum Bulb | Nature's Sunlight | ||
UV blocking sheet | GAM UV Sheet |