We have previously developed protocols for Caenorhabditis elegans to form short- and long-term associative memories by massed and spaced training, respectively. Here, detailed protocols are described for the conditioning of C. elegans by pairing 1-propanol and hydrochloric acid as conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, respectively, to form aversive associative memory.
The nematode Caenorhabditis elegans is an attractive model organism to study learning and memory at molecular and cellular levels because of the simplicity of its nervous system, whose chemical and electrical wiring diagrams were completely reconstructed from serial electron micrographs of thin sections. Here, we describe detailed protocols for the conditioning of C. elegans by massed and spaced training for the formation of short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM), respectively. By pairing 1-propanol and hydrochloric acid as conditioned and unconditioned stimuli, respectively, C. elegans was successfully trained to form aversive associative STM and LTM. While naïve animals were attracted to 1-propanol, the trained animals were no longer or very weakly attracted to 1-propanol. Like in other organisms such as Aplysia and Drosophila, “learning and memory genes” play essential roles in memory formation. Particularly, NMDA-type glutamate receptors, expressed in only six pairs of interneurons in C. elegans, are required for the formation of both STM and LTM, possibly as a coincidence factor. Therefore, the memory trace may reside among the interneurons.
Learning and memory are vital for animals to survive and reproduce by efficiently navigating changing environments. C. elegans is an attractive model organism to study learning and memory at the molecular and cellular levels because of the simplicity of its nervous system, whose chemical and electrical wiring diagrams were completely reconstructed from serial electron micrographs of thin sections1,2,3.
C. elegans learns to associate cultivation temperature with starvation and migrates away from its growth temperature with an aversive memory lasting for several hours4,5. Conditioning C. elegans with sodium chloride (NaCl) in the absence of food leads to a reduction in chemotaxis toward NaCl6,7,8. When paired with food, butanone attraction is enhanced as a result of appetitive learning9,10,11. Although these phenomena are interpreted as associative learning and memory10,12, the distinction between associative learning and non-associative sensitization, habituation, and adaptation is not clear in the C. elegans learning and memory paradigm13,14. Indeed, animals conditioned with butanone and food deprivation (aversive conditioning) showed depressed coupling of the butanone sensory neuron AWCON to target neurons by insulin signals from other neurons, including AIA interneurons, while animals conditioned with butanone and food (appetitive conditioning) showed enhanced coupling of AWCON to target neurons15. The insulin signaling causes gene expression changes induced by nuclear EGL-4 and other transcriptional regulators16,17. Thus, this aversive and appetitive learning and memory has analogies with non-associative habituation and sensitization, respectively, of presynaptic sensory neurons in the gill-withdrawal reflex in Aplysia18,19.
By pairing two chemicals as the conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditioned stimulus (US), we and others have developed protocols for the conditioning of C. elegans to form associative learning and memory without using food or starvation as the US20,21,22,23. In the present study, the protocols are modified to condition animals with 1-propanol and hydrochloric acid (HCl, pH 4.0) as the CS and US, respectively, for aversive learning and short-term memory (STM) and long-term memory (LTM). Naïve C. elegans is attracted by 1-propanol24 and repelled by acid25. When conditioned with a mixture of 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0), C. elegans was no longer or very weakly attracted to 1-propanol.
1. Recipes
2. Preparation of synchronized C. elegans
3. Mass training for short-term associative learning and memory
NOTE: See Figure 1 for the massed training workflow.
4. Spaced training for long-term associative learning and memory
NOTE: See Figure 2 for spaced training workflow.
5. Chemotaxis assay
C. elegans was conditioned by massed training to form short-term aversive associative memory by pairing 1% aqueous 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0) as the CS and US, respectively. According to the protocol described above, synchronized animals were cultivated on a bench at an RT of 18 °C for 5 days and were very gently washed 2x with ddH2O at an RT of 18 °C. Then, the animals were conditioned with a mixture of 1% aqueous 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0) for 1 s. We also trained animals with ddH2O only, 1% aqueous 1-propanol only, and HCl (pH 4.0) only as references. After the conditioning, the animals were washed 1x with ddH2O. We repeated the conditioning 10x without interruption (no ITIs). Successful conditioning was achieved by repeating the procedure more than 7x up to 10x. Conditioning more than 10x resulted in less efficient learning21. After the training, the animals rested on bacterial food for 10 min at RT (18 °C). After being washed with ddH2O 3x, the animals were transferred to a microcentrifuge tube by suspending in 0.25% aqueous gelatin and settled down to the bottom by gravity. After removing the supernatant as much as possible, the animals were gently resuspended in chemotaxis assay buffer and then allowed to settle down to the bottom of the tube by gravity.
After removing as much supernatant as possible, the animal suspension was spotted on the center circle of a chemotaxis assay plate, which was kept at an RT of 18 °C, and then animals were allowed to freely move on the plate for 10 min at an RT of 18 °C. C.I. values were calculated using the equation shown in Figure 3B. As shown in Figure 4A, animals conditioned with the mixture of 1% 1-propanol and HCl were no longer attracted to 5% 1-propanol spotted on agar plates for chemotaxis assay, whereas naïve and reference animals were similarly attracted to 5% 1-propanol. After the massed training (step 3.), the memory was no longer observed within 3 h20. Furthermore, the memory formed by the massed training was sensitive to cold shock20. These results demonstrate that C. elegans successfully formed aversive STM by massed training.
Animals were also conditioned by spaced training 10x with a 10 min ITI between the training steps (step 4.). During the ITI, the collector with animals was placed on a bacterial lawn on a 6 cm NGM plate at an RT of 18 °C. Animals conditioned by the spaced training with a mixture of 1% aqueous 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0) were no longer attracted to 5% 1-propanol in comparison to animals treated with 1% 1-propanol only, HCl (pH 4.0) only, or ddH2O only (Figure 4B). After the spaced training, the animals retained the memory for more than 12 h20,21. Moreover, the memory did not form when animals were treated with translation or transcription inhibitors and was resistant to cold shock20,21. Therefore, C. elegans successfully formed aversive LTM by spaced training.
We also examined the effects of mutations in "learning and memory genes" on the formation of STM and LTM. The crh-1 gene encodes the ubiquitous transcription factor cAMP-response element-binding protein (CREB), glr-1 and nmr-1 encode α-amino-3-hydroxyl-5-methyl-4-isoxazolepropionic acid (AMPA)-type and N-methyl-D-aspartate (NMDA)-type glutamate receptor subunits, respectively, and stau-1 encodes the double-stranded RNA-binding protein Staufen isoform. These genes play essential roles in classical conditioning in C. elegans, Drosophila, Aplysia, and mice. Using a mixture of 1% aqueous 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0), the formation of STM and LTM was dependent on all the genes (Figures 5A,B).
Figure 1: Experimental schematic of massed training. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 2: Experimental schematic of spaced training. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 3: Chemotaxis assay and chemotaxis index. (A) Schematic representation of a chemotaxis assay plate. Petri dishes (6 cm in diameter) were separated into four areas as shown, and 4 μL each of 5% aqueous 1-propanol or ddH2O were diagonally spotted at two places each, 2 cm away from the center. (B) Chemotaxis index values were calculated from the equation shown by counting the number of animals in areas "a" and "b" after the completion of the chemotaxis. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4: Chemotaxis index values of animals conditioned with chemicals. Synchronized wild-type N2 animals were conditioned with chemicals indicated by (A) massed training 10x or (B) spaced training 10x. Flowcharts of the massed and spaced training protocols used are shown in Figure 1 and Figure 2, respectively. After the conditioning, the animals were free to move for 10 min on a 6 cm agar plate for chemotaxis assay at an RT of 18 °C. C.I. values were calculated using the equation shown in Figure 3B. Data for this figure are provided in Supplemental Table 1. Data from the naïve animals were replotted in both figure panels. Bar plot shows the 1st quartile, median, and 3rd quartile. Asterisks (*P < 0.05) indicate statistically significant differences determined by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett's multiple comparison test. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 5: Learning index values of conditioned mutant animals. Synchronized wild-type N2 and mutant animals indicated were conditioned with a mixture of 1% aqueous 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0) by (A) massed training 10x or (B) spaced training 10x. Flowcharts of the massed and spaced training protocols used are shown in Figure 1 and Figure 2, respectively. After the conditioning, the animals were free to move for 10 min on a 6 cm agar plate for chemotaxis assay at an RT of 18 °C. Data for this figure are provided in Supplemental Table 2. Bar plot shows the 1st quartile, median, and 3rd quartile. Asterisks (*P < 0.05) indicate statistically significant differences determined by one-way ANOVA followed by Dunnett's multiple comparison test. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Supplemental Figure 1: Young adult animals are sensitive to chemical treatment. Day 4 and day 5 wild-type N2 animals after hatching were massed-trained 10x with HCl, pH 4.0, without interruption and were then assayed for chemotaxis to 5% aqueous 1-propanol. Bars are means ± S.E.M. (n = 19). Asterisks (*P < 0.05) indicate statistically significant differences determined by two-way ANOVA followed by the Tukey-Kramer post-hoc test. Please click here to download this File.
Supplemental Table 1: Data corresponding to Figure 4. Please click here to download this Table.
Supplemental Table 2: Data corresponding to Figure 5. Please click here to download this Table.
In the present study, all the reagents were kept at an RT of ~18 °C on average, and animals were cultivated on a bench at the RT to avoid stress to the animals. Furthermore, all the experimental procedures were carried out at the RT. Animals were initially cultivated in an incubator at 20 °C and then conditioned on a bench at ~24 °C using reagents at the RT. Under these conditions, the outcomes of the conditioning were very variable. At the low RT, C. elegans grows slowly and should be cultivated longer than at 20 °C until the animals reach the mature adulthood stage, as younger adult animals are more sensitive to the chemicals used for conditioning than mature adult animals and may show lower C.I. values.
The most critical step for successful conditioning is the washing of animals with ddH2O immediately after each chemical treatment. Therefore, mechanical and temperature stresses should be minimized by using sawed-off pipet tips, keeping reagents at RT, and very gently washing the animals by very slowly moving the animal collector up and down in ddH2O. Thorough washing of the animals each time after conditioning may affect learning and memory. The conditions of the chemotaxis assay plates also severely affect the outcomes. Too dry or too wet plates prevent smooth locomotion of the animals. Plates were prepared as described in step 1.; a good plate is one for which the 4 µL spots of ddH2O or 5% aqueous 1-propanol are completely absorbed by the agar in approximately 5 min after spotting. As described above, animal ages are also critical for successful conditioning. Young adult animals are sensitive to mechanical and chemical treatment, resulting in variable outcomes, although very aged animals may not be suitable for conditioning either.
The shelf life of 1-propanol depends on brands and lots and is less than 3 months at RT. When the C.I. values of naïve animals get worse, it would be recommended to use fresh 1-propanol for the conditioning and chemotaxis assay.
The formation of memory by massed training was not affected by the treatment of animals with translation inhibitors (cycloheximide and anisomycin) and a transcription inhibitor (actinomycin D), while the formation of memory by the spaced training was markedly inhibited by the inhibitors20,21. Furthermore, the former memory decayed by cold shock, while the latter was retained for a longer period than the former and was resistant to cold shock. These results demonstrate that the former is STM and the latter is LTM, respectively20,21. However, the memory formed by the massed training may consist of STM and middle-term (intermediate-term) memory since STM is weakly dependent upon the CREB transcription factor (Figure 5A). This is consistent with the result that the STM was retained for more than 1 h20,21. The formation of both STM and LTM is highly dependent on nmr-1, which is expressed only in six pairs of neurons (AVA, AVD, AVE, RIM, AVG, and PVC) in C. elegans27,28. In these neurons, therefore, NMDA receptors may act as a molecular coincidence detector of 1% aqueous 1-propanol and HCl (pH 4.0) signals for synaptic plasticity, where the synaptic strengthening required for both STM and LTM can result from coincidental firing of pre- and post-synaptic neurons29,30,31,32,33. Therefore, the aversive associative memory may form among the interneurons.
The methods described in the present study should be applicable for appetitive olfactory learning and short-term and long-term associative memory using 1-nonanol as the CS and potassium chloride as the US21. It is interesting to compare the neuronal circuits that are involved in the formation of appetitive and aversive memories.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
We are grateful to Takashi Murayama, Ei-ichiro Saita, Iou Ven Chang, and Hitomi Ohtaki for their technical assistance and comments on the manuscript. Strains were provided by the Caenorhabditis Genetics Center, which is funded by the NIH National Center for Research Resources (NCRR). This work was supported by funding from Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University.
500 mL beaker | HARIO | B-500-H32 | |
10 µL pipette tips | Thermo Fisher Scientific | H-104-96RS-Q | |
0.2 mL pipette tips | Thermo Fisher Scientific | TTW110RS-Q | |
1.0 mL pipette tips | Thermo Fisher Scientific | H-111-R100NS-Q | |
1.5 mL plastic tubes | Eppendorf | 0030120086 | |
2 mL plastic tubes | Eppendorf | 0030120094 | |
10 mL Serological pipettes | As One | 2-5237-04 | |
50 mL Serological pipettes | As One | 2-5237-06 | |
6-well cell culture plate | Costar | 3516 | |
Aron Alpha (Glue for plastic) | Toagosei | High Speed EX | |
Autoclave | Tomy Digital Biology | SX-300 | |
Bacto agar | BD | 214010 | |
Bacto peptone | BD | 211677 | |
Bottle top 0.2 µm filter units | Thermo Fisher Scientific | 566-0020 | |
Bunsen burner | EISCO | SKU CH0089A | |
Calcium chloride dihydrate | Nacalai Tesque | 06730-15 | |
C. elegans mutant strains | Caenorhabditis Genetics Center | ||
Cholesterol | Wako Pure Chemical Industries | 034-03002 | |
Clear acrylic cylindrical pipe | Asahi Kasei | 3.5 cm (length), 30 mm (external diameter), 2 mm (thickness) | |
Crystallizing dish | Pyrex | 3140-80 | |
Dental burner | Phoenix-Dent | APT-3 | |
Di-potassium hydrogen phosphate | Nacalai Tesque | 28726-05 | |
E. coli OP50 | Caenorhabditis Genetics Center | ||
Electric pipetter | Drummond Scientific | 4-000-101 | |
Gelatin | Wako Pure Chemical Industries | 073-06295 | |
Glass Petri dishes (10 cm in diameter) | As One | Trade FLAT Mark | |
Heating magnetic stirrer | Thermo Fisher Scientific | SP131324 | |
Hydrochloric acid | Nacalai Tesque | 37345-15 | |
Incubator | SANYO | MIR-553 | |
Kimwipes S-200 | Nippon Paper Crecia | 62011 | |
Laboratory coat | TOYO LINT FREE | FH240C | |
Magnesium sulfate heptahydrate | Nacalai Tesque | 21002-85 | |
Magnetic stirrer bar | SANSYO | 93-5412 | |
Metal spatula | FUJIFILM Wako | 647-06531 | |
Nitrile gloves | Kimberly-Clark | KC100 | |
Nylon mesh (mesh size: 30 μm) | SEFAR | NY30-HD | |
P10 pipetman | Gilson | F144802 | |
P200 pipetman | Gilson | F123600 | |
P1000 pipetman | Gilson | F123602 | |
pH meter | HORIBA | Navi F-52 | |
Plastic Petri dishes (9 cm in diameter) | IWAKI | SH90-15E | |
Plastic Petri dishes (6 cm in diameter) | SARSTEDT | 82.1194.500 | |
Plastic weighing boats | As One | 1-5233-01 | |
Platinum wire for a worm pick | Nilaco | PT-351265 | |
1-Propanol | SIGMA-ALDRICH | 279544 | |
Potassium dihydrogen phosphate | Nacalai Tesque | 28721-55 | |
Safety goggles | Kimberly-Clark | #25646 | |
Sodium chloride | Nacalai Tesque | 31320-05 | |
Stereomicroscope | Olympus | SZX16 | |
Tooth picks | |||
Water purification sysytem | Merck | Elix Essential 10 UV | |
Water urification sysytem | Merck | Milli-Q Synthesis A10 | |
Weighing balance | METTLER | TOREDO | |
Wild type C. elegans strain N2 | Caenorhabditis Genetics Center |