Using two different transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) protocols, this manuscript describes how to measure and compare cortical inhibition within the primary motor cortex when adopting different attentional foci.
It is well recognized that an external focus (EF) compared with an internal focus (IF) of attention improves motor learning and performance. Studies have indicated benefits in accuracy, balance, force production, jumping performance, movement speed, oxygen consumption, and fatiguing task. Although behavioral outcomes of using an EF strategy are well explored, the underlying neural mechanisms remain unknown. A recent TMS study compared the activity of the primary motor cortex (M1) between an EF and an IF. More precisely, this study showed that, when adopting an EF, the activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits is enhanced.
On the behavioral level, the present protocol tests the influence of attentional foci on the time to task failure (TTF) when performing submaximal contractions of the first dorsal interosseous (FDI). Additionally, the current paper describes two TMS protocols to assess the influence of attentional conditions on the activity of cortical inhibitory circuits within the M1. Thus, the present article describes how to use single-pulse TMS at intensities below the motor threshold (subTMS) and paired-pulse TMS, inducing short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) when applied to the M1. As these methods are assumed to reflect the responsiveness of GABAergic inhibitory neurons, without being affected by spinal reflex circuitries, they are well suited to measuring the activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits within the M1.
The results show that directing attention externally improves motor performance, as participants were able to prolong the time to task failure. Moreover, the results were accompanied by a larger subTMS-induced electromyography suppression and SICI when adopting an EF compared to an IF. As the level of cortical inhibition within the M1 was previously demonstrated to influence motor performance, the enhanced inhibition with an EF might contribute to the better movement efficiency observed in the behavioral task, indicated by a prolonged TTF with an EF.
It is now generally accepted that adopting an EF compared to an IF or neutral focus of attention promotes motor performance and learning in numerous settings1. It has been shown, for example, that adopting an EF leads to benefits in accuracy2,3, balance4,5,6, force production7,8, jumping performance7,9,10,11, movement speed12, oxygen consumption13,14, and fatiguing tasks15,16.
On the other side, since brain activation is the basis of all movements, several aspects of the neural control of movement have been investigated. For example, the level and the ability to modulate intracortical inhibition within the M1 has been shown to have a strong influence on motor function, such as interlimb coordination17, postural control18, and dexterity19. Furthermore, populations with poorer motor control abilities than young adults, such as elderly subjects or children (born preterm20), usually show less pronounced inhibitory control. Thus, although the role of inhibitory processes is not yet well understood, inhibitory processes nevertheless seem to be important to the quality of motor execution in general.
A possibility to investigate intracortical inhibitory circuitries is to use non-invasive transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). The most commonly used stimulation protocol applies paired-pulse TMS (ppTMS) to induce SICI. This protocol uses a conditioning stimulus below the motor threshold to reduce the amplitude of the suprathreshold control stimulus response elicited at an interstimulus interval of 1-5 ms21,22,23,24. Then, reported as the percentage of the control stimulus, the amplitudes of the motor-evoked potentials (MEPs) can be compared across conditions, giving information about cortical inhibitory activity and modulation within the M1.
Another stimulation protocol to assess the activity of intractortical inhibitory circuits applies single pulses, where all stimuli are delivered at intensities below the motor threshold (i.e., subTMS). This protocol induces suppression in the ongoing EMG activity18,25,26. This so-called subTMS-induced EMG suppression can be compared in terms of amount and duration. Although this protocol is not so commonly used, it has certain advantages compared to the standard SICI protocol. This protocol does not disturb motor execution, as it does not induce suprathreshold stimuli. Both methods test the responsiveness of intracortical gamma-aminobutyric acid (GABA) inhibitory interneurons23,27.
Despite the well-known benefits of using an EF compared to an IF on motor performance1, the underlying neural processes remain largely unknown. In a former fMRI study28, it was shown that blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) activation was enhanced in the M1, primary somatosensory, and insular cortices when subjects executed a finger sequence and adopted an EF compared to an IF. As excitatory and inhibitory activity cannot be differentiated by fMRI29, another recent study16 stipulated that the enhanced activity in the M1 associated with an EF could, in fact, be due to the enhanced activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits. More precisely, this study showed that the excitability of inhibitory GABAergic neurons can be modulated instantly by the type of attentional focus adopted in one and the same person.
The main aim of the present protocol is to show two possible ways to compare the immediate effects of cognitive manipulation (i.e., focus of attention instructions) on the activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits within the M1. SubTMS and ppTMS are both used. In addition, this protocol shows one possible way to explore the influence of attentional foci on motor behavior in a very controlled way by investigating the TTF of submaximal isometric sustained contraction of the FDI.
This protocol was approved by the local ethics committee, and the experiments are in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964).
1. Ethical Approval and Subject Instruction
2. Experimental Design and Setup
3. Subject Preparation
4. Sessions 1 and 2: Behavioral Tests
5. Sessions 3 and 4: Brain Stimulation
6. Data Processing and Analysis
The Influence of Attentional Foci on Motor Performance:
The behavioral tests in the current study were used to prove the feasibility of the motor task and to identify the subjects who reacted positively when applying an EF. In line with previous studies (see1 for a review), our results show a prolonged TTF when the participants adopted an EF compared to an IF (see Figure 3). Thus, it seems that, during an isometric index finger abduction, the efficiency of the movement can be enhanced by an EF. McNevin and colleagues38 posited the "constrained action hypothesis" to explain the effects of different foci of attention on motor performance and motor learning. The authors posited in their hypothesis: that using an EF ameliorates motor performance by promoting a greater automaticity in movement control. In contrast, the adoption of an IF is supposed to constrain the motor system, as a more conscious type of motor control is used. Nonetheless, despite the well-known benefits of using an EF compared to an IF on motor performance in general1, the underlying neural processes remain poorly investigated. Therefore, the central question remains: determining how the enhanced movement efficiency associated with an EF compared to an IF is controlled from a motor cortical point of view.
Intracortical Inhibition and Motor Abilities:
Cortical activity is constituted of interactions between excitatory and inhibitory mechanisms within brain motor areas24. In addition, the modulation of these processes are essential for motor control39. For example, children40,41,42 and elderly individuals43 show reduced levels of intracortical inhibition—in contrast to healthy, young subjects—resulting in reduced coordinative abilities. In general, it seems that intracortical inhibitory processes and motor performance are closely interrelated when considering different populations. In addition, not only across age groups or different populations, but also within age groups, motor function seems to be strongly altered by corticospinal inhibitory processes, such as interlimb coordination17 or dexterity19. Therefore, the level of intracortical inhibition within the M1 seems to affect the characteristics of motor control in general.
The Measure and Influence of Attentional Foci on Intracortical Inhibition:
In a previous fMRI study, Zentgraf and coworkers28 started to investigate neural correlates associated with attentional foci (i.e., EF versus IF). The results showed greater activation in different brain areas—the M1, the insular, and the primary somatosensory cortices—when subjects performed a keyboard finger sequence in an EF condition rather than an IF condition. Apart from the limitation that different subjects were investigated in the EF and IF tasks, making direct comparisons impossible, the fMRI technique is not able to differentiate between excitatory and inhibitory neural activity29, as it uses intrinsic blood-tissue contrasts44. Therefore, the higher brain activation found in the M1 in the EF condition demonstrated in this previous fMRI study28 may result from increased excitatory or inhibitory activity. Therefore, fMRI provides only an estimate about the overall neural activity29. In contrast and in complement to fMRI, TMS can give information about the nature of the enhanced activity, whether it results from excitatory or inhibitory activity. The reason for this is that TMS applied to the M1 at intensities below the active motor threshold inhibit motor cortical output, as the cortical inhibitory GABAergic interneurons have a lower threshold to TMS than the excitatory neurons27,45,46,47,48. In addition, it was shown that TMS under the motor threshold does not cause descending volleys and, therefore, does not activate spinal structures23,27. In this study, we used two TMS protocols to measure the cortical inhibition within M1. The first used a single-pulse subTMS protocol, which induces a suppression in the ongoing EMG activity. It has been proposed that the inhibition of the ongoing activity of fast-conducting corticospinal cells results in a subTMS-induced EMG suppression49.
Thus, there is a relationship between the excitability of intracortical inhibitory circuits and the amount of subTMS-induced EMG suppression. In other words, an increase in cortical inhibition within the M1 results in more EMG suppression18. Although the subTMS protocol is not so widely used, it inherits many advantages compared to protocols using suprathresold stimuli: first, as the stimulation does not add but rather removes activity from the descending corticospinal volley, the effects can clearly be attributed to the primary motor cortex, as they are not affected by spinal circuitries23,27. Second, as subthreshold intensities are used, no muscle twitch in induced by stimulation, which may disturb motor performance. Using this technique, we demonstrated that the subTMS-induced EMG suppression was instantly enhanced when using an EF compared to an IF (see Figure 4 for results and analysis). Specifically, our results showed that the activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits within the M1 is immediately modulated when different attentional foci are adopted.
Another more widespread possibility for measuring the activity of GABAergic motor interneurons is to apply a ppTMS paradigm with short interstimulus intervals over the contralateral M1. The paired-pulse stimulation induces a reduction in the MEP amplitude, which is called the SICI, and reflects the activity of inhibitory GABAergic neurons21,45,50.
When adopting an EF, participants showed more SICI (see Figure 5 for results and analysis). This is well in line with the subTMS results and suggests that GABAergic neurons, constituting the intracortical inhibitory circuits51, are modulated differently within the M1 according to the type of attentional focus. This would be in line with former research showing that the M1 is sensitive to differential attentional situations52. In addition, as a positive correlation between the cerebral blood flow in the motor cortex and the amount of SICI has been revealed in a positron emission tomography study53, our results might further support the enhanced cortical activity within the M1 that was found by Zentgraf and colleagues28. Finally, as the motor tasks and background EMG prior to stimulation were similar in both conditions, it has been deduced that verbal instructions stipulating the direction of attention indeed have a main modulatory influence on the activity of the intracortical inhibitory neurons projecting to the FDI.
Figure 1. Time course of the four protocols. A. The aim of the first two sessions (S1 and S2) is to compare the time to task failure (TTF) of a submaximal sustained abduction of the right index finger at 30% of the Fmax between an external (EF) and an internal focus of attention (IF). During the EF session, the subjects are asked to concentrate on the goniometer angle (i.e., the movement effect), while during the IF session, they are asked to concentrate on their index finger and muscle (i.e., body movement). B. The third and fourth sessions (S3 and S4) aim to compare the cortical activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits within the M1 between an EF an IF. This can be achieved by comparing the amount and the duration of subthreshold TMS (subTMS) induced EMG suppression and by comparing the amount of short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI) induced by paired-pulse TMS (ppTMS). This figure was adapted from Kuhn et al16. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 2. Experimental setup. A. 1. The TMS coil is placed over the contralateral M1 over the hand representation. 2. The participant's forhead and the TMS coil are mounted with reflecting markers to control the position of the TMS coil relative to the skull. 3. The orthopedic splint restricts movement of the wrist and only allows movements of the index finger. 4. EMG electrodes are placed in a tendon-belly montage over the FDI. 5. The goniometer calculates the angle of the metacarpophalangeal joint of the index finger. 6. The weight representing either 30% (S1 and S2) or the 10% (S3 and S4) of Fmax is attached to the rope. B. The movements of the metacarpophalangeal joint are displayed on a computer screen placed 1 m in front of the subject. When the angle is 90°, the red line displayed on the computer screen is the thinnest. As soon as the participant's finger moves to the left or right, the red line gets thicker in the corresponding direction. The aim of the motor task is to keep the red line as thin as possible. To measure the Fmax (S1 and S2), the force transducer is placed (1) such that the participants can push against it (i.e., isometric contraction), keeping a constant angle of 90°. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 3. Time to task failure (TTF) of sustained contractions. The TTF was prolonged (approximately by +18%) when the participants (n = 14) adopted an external (EF) rather than an internal focus of attention (IF). * p < 0.05. The error bars represent the SEM. This figure was adapted from Kuhn et al16. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 4. SubTMS-induced EMG suppression. A.To acquire the curves of the mean EMG activity during the sustained contraction of the right first dorsal interosseous (FDI) at 10% of Fmax, the rectified EMG (full-wave rectification) of the trials with subTMS is subtracted from that of the trials without stimulation. The vertical lines represent (1) the onset of subTMS-induced EMG suppression and (2) the end of subTMS-induced EMG suppression. B. Representative data (n = 10) of the amount of the subTMS-induced EMG suppression. The data are obtained by computing the cumulative trapezoidal numerical integration from the onset to the end of the suppression (i.e., the negative area under each curve from 1 to 2 in A). The amount of subTMS-induced EMG suppression is enhanced when an external focus (EF) rather than an internal focus of attention (IF) is adopted. C. Representative data (n = 10) of the subTMS-induced EMG suppression duration from 1 to 2. No significant difference was found in the duration of the suppression, but it is longer with an EF. Thus, it is reasonable to assume that the effect size was too small to induce a significant difference in our relatively small sample size. ** p < 0.01. The error bars represent the SEM. This figure was adapted from Kuhn et al16. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
Figure 5. Short-interval intracortical inhibition (SICI). A. The SICI is expressed as the percentage of the control MEP in FDI by applying the following formula: 100 – (conditioned MEP / control MEP × 100). The SICI is enhanced when the participants adopt an EF compared to an IF. This reflects greater activation of intracortical inhibitory circuits. B. As the amplitude of the control MEP has an influence on the size of the conditioned MEP, the control MEPs at 1.2 aMT peak-to-peak amplitudes should be compared between the two conditions (i.e., EF versus IF). ** p < 0.01. The error bars represent the SEM. This figure was adapted from Kuhn et al16. Please click here to view a larger version of this figure.
This protocol shows two possible methods to investigate the activity of inhibitory circuits within the M1 using TMS. More precisely, these two protocols have been used in this study to investigate the impact of attentional foci on the activity of inhibitory circuits within the M1.
One limitation of the presented method is that it is not always possible to cause a subTMS-induced EMG suppression without a facilitation preceding it. In this study, for example, four subjects had to be removed from the final analysis, as they did not show any consistent subTMS-induced EMG suppression. Nonetheless, this non-invasive brain stimulation method is well accepted for measuring and quantifying the activity of intracortical inhibitory circuits within the M132,34. Another limitation of this study is that it cannot be excluded that the differences between the foci of attention outlined by subTMS and ppTMS rely on brain areas upstream the M1. Despite the fact that both methods are assumed to test the responsiveness of intracortical GABA inhibitory interneurons23,27, there is no correlation between the amount of subTMS-induced EMG suppression and the amount of SICI16; further investigations are needed.
In addition, it is important to use a light resistance (10% of Fmax) during the TMS protocols, to conduct the subTMS experiment in separate sessions (≥ 72-h break), and to randomize the conditions. The main reason is that fatigue can influence the magnitude of subTMS-induced EMG suppression32 and the level of SICI54, meaning that the main effect of attention might be biased by fatigue. During a fatiguing task, a number of peripheral, subcortical, and cortical mechanisms can also play a crucial role in performance. Moreover, it is important to use a neuronavigation system, as the TMS coil must be placed in the same spot before each trial. In addition, this system allows the experimenter to check the coil position at any time throughout the whole experiment.
The main finding of the present study is that cortical inhibition within the M1 can be affected instantly in the same subject according to the attentional focus adopted during the motor execution. As inhibitory processes seem to be closely related to the quality of motor execution in general, our results might explain on a neural level the enhanced efficiency of an EF compared to an IF. It can be speculated that the increased level of inhibition during EF avoids unnecessary co-activity and leads to a more focal activation, resulting in a more efficient motor execution. In this way, our results might constitute one of the underlying mechanisms of the "constrained action hypothesis." In addition, this protocol is the first to show how to apply subTMS and ppTMS to the same participants using a repeated-measures design. Moreover, despite the fact that a large number of studies show that adopting an EF compared to an IF promotes motor performance and learning in numerous settings1, only very few investigate the underlying neural mechanisms when different attentional situations stipulated through verbal instruction are adopted16,28,55.
The authors have nothing to disclose.
The authors have no acknowledgements.
MC3A-100 | Advanced Mechanical Technologies Inc., Watertown, MA, USA | – | Force transducer |
BlueSensor P | Ambu A/S, Bellerup, Denmark | – | Ag/AgCl surface electrodes for EMG |
Polaris Spectra | Northern Digital, Waterloo, ON, Canada | – | neuronavigation system, active or passive markers tracker |
Localite TMS Navigator Version 2.0.5 | LOCALITE GmbH, Sankt Augustin, Germany | – | navigation system for transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) |
MagVenture MagPro X100 | MagVenture A/S, Farum, Denmark | 9016E0711 | Transcranial magnetic stimulator |
MagVenture D-B80 | MagVenture A/S, Farum, Denmark | 9016E0431 | TMS coil (figure of eight) |
Goniometer | N/A | – | Custom-made goniometer |
Othopedic splint | N/A | – | Custom-made splint |
Recording software | LabView based | – | Custom-made script |